751
Views
15
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The dual origin of semantic errors in access deficit: activation vs. inhibition deficit

Pages 31-53 | Received 04 May 2018, Accepted 19 Feb 2019, Published online: 08 Mar 2019

References

  • Abdel Rahman, R., & Melinger, A. (2009). Semantic context effects in language production: A swinging lexical network proposal and a review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(5), 713–734. doi:10.1080/01690960802597250
  • Ackerman, T., & Ellis, A. W. (2007). Case study: Where do aphasic perseverations come from? Aphasiology, 21(10-11), 1018–1038. doi: 10.1080/02687030701198361
  • Allen, C. M., Martin, R. C., & Martin, N. (2012). Relations between short-term memory deficits, semantic processing, and executive function. Aphasiology, 26(3-4), 428–461. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.617436
  • Anders, R., Riès, S., Van Maanen, L., & Alario, F. X. (2017). Lesions to the left lateral prefrontal cortex impair decision threshold adjustment for lexical selection. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(1-2), 1–20. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1282447
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47–89. doi: 10.1016/s0079-7421(08)60452-1
  • Bahlmann, J., Blumenfeld, R. S., & D'esposito, M. (2014). The rostro-caudal axis of frontal cortex is sensitive to the domain of stimulus information. Cerebral Cortex, 25(7), 1815–1826. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht419
  • Belke, E., Meyer, A. S., & Damian, M. F. (2005). Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(4), 667–692. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000142
  • Belke, E., Shao, Z., & Meyer, A. S. (2017). Strategic origins of early semantic facilitation in the blocked-cyclic naming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(10), 1659. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000399
  • Belke, E., & Stielow, A. (2013). Cumulative and non-cumulative semantic interference in object naming: Evidence from blocked and continuous manipulations of semantic context. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 2135–2160. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.775318
  • Bloem, I., & La Heij, W. (2003). Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: Implications for models of lexical access in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(3), 468–488. doi: 10.1037/e501882009-509
  • Bloem, I., van den Boogaard, S., & La Heij, W. (2004). Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in language production: Further evidence for the conceptual selection model of lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 307–323. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.001
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.43 Retrieved from May 2018.
  • Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (1998). Competition between past and present. Assessment and Interpretation of Verbal Perseverations. Brain: a Journal of Neurology, 121(9), 1641–1659. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.9.1641
  • Corbett, F., Jefferies, E., Ehsan, S., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2009). Different impairments of semantic cognition in semantic dementia and semantic aphasia: Evidence from the non-verbal domain. Brain, 132(9), 2593–2608. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp146
  • Costa, A., Alario, F. X., & Caramazza, A. (2005). On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(1), 125–131. doi: 10.3758/bf03196357
  • Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., & Porter, S. (2010). Point and interval estimates of effect sizes for the case-controls design in neuropsychology: Rationale, methods, implementations, and proposed reporting standards. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27(3), 245–260. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2010.513967
  • Damian, M. F., & Als, L. C. (2005). Long-lasting semantic context effects in the spoken production of object names. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6), 1372. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1372
  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.93.3.283
  • Dell, G. S., Burger, L. K., & Svec, W. R. (1997b). Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model. Psychological Review, 104(1), 123. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.104.1.123
  • Dell, G. S., Nozari, N., & Oppenheim, G. M. (2014). Word production: Behavioral and computational considerations. In M. A. Goldrick, V. S. Ferreira, & M. Miozzo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language production (pp. 88–104). Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199735471.013.014.
  • Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997a). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104(4), 801. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.104.4.801
  • Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Nozari, N., Faseyitan, O., & Coslett, H. B. (2013). Voxel-based lesion-parameter mapping: Identifying the neural correlates of a computational model of word production. Cognition, 128(3), 380–396. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.007
  • de Zubicaray, G., Fraser, D., Ramajoo, K., & McMahon, K. (2017). Interference from related actions in spoken word production: Behavioural and fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia, 96, 78–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.010
  • de Zubicaray, G., McMahon, K., & Howard, D. (2015). Perfusion fMRI evidence for priming of shared feature-to-lexical connections during cumulative semantic interference in spoken word production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(3), 261–272. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2013.848990
  • Fedorenko, E., Behr, M. K., & Kanwisher, N. (2011). Functional specificity for high-level linguistic processing in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(39), 16428–16433. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112937108
  • Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza, A. (2006). Now you see it, now you don't: On turning semantic interference into facilitation in a Stroop-like task. Cortex, 42(6), 790–796. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70419-2
  • Fischer-Baum, S., McCloskey, M., & Rapp, B. (2010). Representation of letter position in spelling: Evidence from acquired dysgraphia. Cognition, 115(3), 466–490. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.013
  • Fischer-Baum, S., & Rapp, B. (2012). Underlying cause (s) of letter perseveration errors. Neuropsychologia, 50(2), 305–318. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.001
  • Freedman, M. L., & Martin, R. C. (2001). Dissociable components of short-term memory and their relation to long-term learning. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(3), 193–226. doi: 10.1080/02643290042000080
  • Freund, M., Gordon, B., & Nozari, N. (2016). Conflict-based regulation of control in language production. In A. Papafragou, D. Grodner, D. Mirman, & J. C. Trueswell (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the cognitive Science Society (pp. 1625–1630). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Freund, M., & Nozari, N. (2018). Is adaptive control in language production mediated by learning? Cognition, 176, 107–130. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.03.009
  • Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2005). Dissociations among tasks involving inhibition: A single-case study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(1), 1–13.
  • Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (2007). Semantic dementia: A unique clinicopathological syndrome. The Lancet Neurology, 6(11), 1004–1014. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(07)70266-1
  • Hoffman, P., McClelland, J. L., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2018). Concepts, control, and context: A connectionist account of normal and disordered semantic cognition. Psychological Review, 125(3), 293. doi: 10.1037/rev0000094
  • Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y
  • Houghton, G., Glasspool, D., & Shallice, T. (1994). Spelling and serial recall: Insights from a competitive queueing model. In G. D. A. Brown, & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0909(199806)4:2%3C112::aid-dys76%3E3.0.co;2-a.
  • Howard, D., Nickels, L., Coltheart, M., & Cole-Virtue, J. (2006). Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: Experimental and computational studies. Cognition, 100(3), 464–482. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006
  • Hsiao, E. Y., Schwartz, M. F., Schnur, T. T., & Dell, G. S. (2009). Temporal characteristics of semantic perseverations induced by blocked-cyclic picture naming. Brain and Language, 108, 133–144. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.11.003
  • Hsu, N. S., & Novick, J. M. (2016). Dynamic engagement of cognitive control modulates recovery from misinterpretation during real-time language processing. Psychological Science, 27(4), 572–582. doi: 10.1177/0956797615625223
  • January, D., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Co-localization of Stroop and syntactic ambiguity resolution in Broca's area: Implications for the neural basis of sentence processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(12), 2434–2444. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21179
  • Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Semantic impairment in stroke aphasia versus semantic dementia: A case-series comparison. Brain, 129(8), 2132–2147. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl153
  • Kuipers, J. R., La Heij, W., & Costa, A. (2006). A further look at semantic context effects in language production: The role of response congruency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(7-8), 892–919. doi: 10.1080/016909600824211
  • Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2017). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(1), 42–55. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.150
  • Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38. doi: 10.1017/s0140525(99001776
  • MacKay, D. (1986). Self-inhibition and the disruptive effects of internal and external feedback in skilled behavior. In H. Heuer, & C. Fromm (Eds.), Generation and modulation of action patterns. London: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-71476-4.
  • Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Why does lexical selection have to be so hard? Comment on Abdel Rahman and Melinger's swinging lexical network proposal. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(5), 735–748. doi:10.1080/01690960802597276
  • Mahon, B. Z., Costa, A., Peterson, R., Vargas, K. A., & Caramazza, A. (2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(3), 503. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503
  • Martin, N., & Dell, G. S. (2007). Common mechanisms underlying perseverative and non-perseverative sound and word substitutions. Aphasiology, 21(10-11), 1002–1017. doi: 10.1080/02687030701198346
  • McCloskey, M., Macaruso, P., & Rapp, B. (2006). Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the spelling system: Evidence from a dysgraphic patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(2), 278–307. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000518
  • Mirman, D., & Britt, A. E. (2014). What we talk about when we talk about access deficits. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120388. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0388
  • Moses, M. S., Sheard, C., & Nickels, L. A. (2007). Insights into recurrent perseverative errors in aphasia: A case series approach. Aphasiology, 21(10-11), 975–1001. doi: 10.1080/02687030701198312
  • Navarrete, E., Del Prato, P., & Mahon, B. Z. (2012). Factors determining semantic facilitation and interference in the cyclic naming paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 38. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00038
  • Navarrete, E., Del Prato, P., Peressotti, F., & Mahon, B. Z. (2014). Lexical selection is not by competition: Evidence from the blocked naming paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 76, 253–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.05.003
  • Noonan, K. A., Jefferies, E., Corbett, F., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). Elucidating the nature of deregulated semantic cognition in semantic aphasia: Evidence for the roles of prefrontal and temporo-parietal cortices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(7), 1597–1613. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21289
  • Nozari, N., & Dell, G. S. (2013). How damaged brains repeat words: A computational approach. Brain and Language, 126(3), 327–337. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.07.005
  • Nozari, N., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2011). Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.05.001.
  • Nozari, N., Freund, M., Breining, B., Rapp, B., & Gordon, B. (2016). Cognitive control during selection and repair in word production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(7), 886–903. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1157194
  • Nozari, N., & Hepner, C. (2018). To select or to wait? The importance of criterion setting in debates of competitive lexical selection. Cognitive Neuropsychology. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2018.1476335
  • Nozari, N., Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2010). Naming and repetition in aphasia: Steps, routes, and frequency effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(4), 541–559. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.08.001
  • Nozari, N., & Novick, J. (2017). Monitoring and control in language production. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(5), 403–410. doi: 10.1177/0963721417702419
  • Nozari, N., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2015). Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in processing of words and Sentences. In G. Hickok, & S. L. Small (Eds.), The Neurobiology of language. Academic Press. doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-407794-2.00001-8.
  • Olivella, S., & Shiraito, Y. (2017). Poisbinom: A Faster Implementation of the Poisson-Binomial Distribution. R package version 1.0.1.
  • Oppenheim, G. M., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2010). The dark side of incremental learning: A model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production. Cognition, 114(2), 227–252. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.007
  • Piai, V., Riès, S. K., & Swick, D. (2016). Lesions to lateral prefrontal cortex impair lexical interference control in word production. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 721. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00721
  • Piai, V., Roelofs, A., Jensen, O., Schoffelen, J.-M., & Bonnefond, M. (2014). Distinct patterns of brain Activity Characterise lexical activation and competition in Spoken word production. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e88674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088674
  • Rabovsky, M., Schad, D. J., & Abdel Rahman, R. (2016). Language production is facilitated by semantic richness but inhibited by semantic density: Evidence from picture naming. Cognition, 146, 240–244. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.016
  • Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M. (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review, 107(3), 460. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.107.3.460
  • Riès, S. K., Dhillon, R. K., Clarke, A., King-Stephens, D., Laxer, K. D., Weber, P. B., … Lin, J. J. (2017). Spatiotemporal dynamics of word retrieval in speech production revealed by cortical high-frequency band activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(23), E4530–E4538. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620669114
  • Roach, A., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Grewal, R. S., & Brecher, A. (1996). The Philadelphia naming test: Scoring and rationale. Clinical Aphasiology, 24, 121–133. doi: 10.1037/t56477-000
  • Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107–142. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90041-f
  • Roelofs, A., & Piai, V. (2011). Attention demands of spoken word planning: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 307. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00307
  • Rogers, T. T., Patterson, K., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). Disorders of representation and control in semantic cognition: Effects of familiarity, typicality, and specificity. Neuropsychologia, 76, 220–239. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.015
  • Schnur, T. T. (2014). The persistence of cumulative semantic interference during naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 75, 27–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.04.006
  • Schnur, T. T., Schwartz, M. F., Brecher, A., & Hodgson, C. (2006). Semantic interference during blocked-cyclic naming: Evidence from aphasia. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(2), 199–227. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.10.002
  • Schnur, T. T., Schwartz, M. F., Kimberg, D. Y., Hirshorn, E., Coslett, H. B., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Localizing interference during naming: Convergent neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence for the function of Broca's area. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(1), 322–327. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805874106
  • Schwartz, M. F., Dell, G. S., Martin, N., Gahl, S., & Sobel, P. (2006). A case-series test of the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Evidence from picture naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(2), 228–264. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.10.001
  • Shao, Z., Meyer, A. S., & Roelofs, A. (2013). Selective and nonselective inhibition of competitors in picture naming. Memory & Cognition, 41(8), 1200–1211. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0332-7
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). Electrophysiological evidence that inhibition supports lexical selection in picture naming. brain Research, 1586, 130–142. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.07.009
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Sources of individual differences in the speed of naming objects and actions: The contribution of executive control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 1927–1944. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.670252
  • Shitova, N., Roelofs, A., Schriefers, H., Bastiaansen, M., & Schoffelen, J. M. (2017). Control adjustments in speaking: Electrophysiology of the Gratton effect in picture naming. Cortex, 92, 289–303. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.04.017
  • Sikora, K., Roelofs, A., Hermans, D., & Knoors, H. (2016). Executive control in spoken noun-phrase production: Contributions of updating, inhibiting, and shifting. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(9), 1719–1740. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.037
  • Snyder, H. R., Banich, M. T., & Munakata, Y. (2011). Choosing our words: Retrieval and selection processes recruit shared neural substrates in left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3470–3482. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00023
  • Song, C., Kanai, R., Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Schwarzkopf, D. S., & Rees, G. (2011). Relating inter-individual differences in metacognitive performance on different perceptual tasks. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1787–1792. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.011
  • Thompson-Schill, S. L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., & Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: A reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94(26), 14792–14797. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.26.14792
  • Vitkovitch, M., Kirby, A., & Tyrrell, L. (1996). Patterns of excitation and inhibition in picture naming. Visual Cognition, 3, 61–80. doi: 10.1080/713756730
  • Vitkovitch, M., Rutter, C., & Read, A. (2001). Inhibitory effects during object name retrieval: The effect of interval between prime and target on picture naming responses. British Journal of Psychology, 92(3), 483–506. doi: 10.1348/000712601162301
  • Vitkovitch, M., & Tyrrell, L. (1999). The effects of distractor words on naming pictures at the subordinate level. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 52(4), 905–926. doi: 10.1080/027249899390864
  • Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1983). Category specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106(4), 859–878. doi: 10.1093/neucas/8.3.192
  • Wheeldon, L. R., & Monsell, S. (1994). Inhibition of spoken word production by priming a semantic competitor. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(3), 332–356. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1016

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.