812
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

A developmental account of the role of sequential dependencies in typical and atypical language learners

&
Pages 243-264 | Received 02 Mar 2023, Accepted 23 Oct 2023, Published online: 14 Nov 2023

References

  • Anderson, N. D., & Dell, G. S. (2018). The role of consolidation in learning context-dependent phonotactic patterns in speech and digital sequence production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(14), 3617–3622. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721107115
  • Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(4), 905–924. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072)
  • Benham, S., & Goffman, L. (2022). A longitudinal study of the phonological organisation of novel word forms in children with developmental language disorder. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 24(2), 212–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2021.1975816
  • Benham, S., Goffman, L., & Schweickert, R. (2018). An application of network science to phonological sequence learning in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(9), 2275–2291. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0036
  • Benham, S., Wisler, A., Berlin, J., Wang, J., & Goffman, L (2023). Acoustic and kinematic methods of indexing spatiotemporal stability in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 66(8S), 3026–3037. https://doi.org/10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00290
  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Specific language impairment as a maturational lag: Evidence from longitudinal data on language and motor development. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 29(4), 442–459. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1987.tb02504.x
  • Brumbach, A., & Goffman, L. (2014). Interaction of language processing and motor skill in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(1), 158–171. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0215)
  • Chambers, K. E., Onishi, K. H., & Fisher, C. L. (2003). Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience. Cognition, 87(2), B69–B77. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00233-0
  • Cullinan, M., Gerken, L. A., Glickman, S., Benham, S., & Goffman, L. (2022). The acquisition of a rule-based sound sequence in typically developing 4- to 6-year-old children. Poster presented at The symposium on research in child language disorders. Madison, WI.
  • Dell, G. S., Kelley, A. C., Hwang, S., & Bian, Y. (2021). The adaptable speaker: A theory of implicit learning in language production. Psychological Review, 128(3), 446–487. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000275.
  • Dell, G. S., Martin, N., & Schwartz, M. E. (2007). A case-series test of the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Predicting word repetition from picture naming⋆. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(4), 490–520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.05.007
  • Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(5), 1136–1146. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136
  • Drake, C. (1993). Reproduction of musical rhythms by children, adult musicians, and adult nonmusicians. Perception & Psychophysics, 53(1), 25–33. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211712
  • Evans, J. L., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 52(2), 321–335. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0189)
  • Factor, L., & Goffman, L. (2022). Phonological characteristics of novel gesture production in children with developmental language disorder: Longitudinal findings. Applied Psycholinguistics, 43(2), 333–362. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716421000540
  • Gerken, L. A. (1994). Young children′s representation of prosodic phonology: Evidence from English-speakers′ weak syllable productions. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(1), 19–38. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1002
  • Gerken, L. A. (1996). Prosodic structure in young children’s language production. Language, 72(4), 683–712. https://doi.org/10.2307/416099
  • Gerken, L. A., Figueroa, M., & Goffman, L. (2022). 11-Month-Olds can learn A phonological pattern that adults cannot. Paper presented at the poster presented at The symposium on research in child language disorders, Madison, WI.
  • Gerken, L. A., & Knight, S. (2015). Infants generalize from just (the right) four words. Cognition, 143, 187–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.04.018
  • Gerken, L. A., Plante, E., & Goffman, L. (2021). Not all procedural learning tasks are difficult for adults with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 64(3), 922–934. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00548
  • Gerken, L. A., & Quam, C. (2017). Infant learning is influenced by local spurious generalizations. Developmental Science, n/a-n/a. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12410
  • Gerken, L. A., Quam, C., & Goffman, L. (2019). Adults fail to learn a type of linguistic pattern that is readily learned by infants. Language Learning and Development, 15(4), 279–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2019.1617149
  • Gladfelter, A., & Goffman, L. (2018). Semantic richness and word learning in children with autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science, 21(2). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12543
  • Goffman, L. (1999). Prosodic influences on speech production in children With specific language impairment and speech deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(6), 1499–1517. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4206.1499
  • Goffman, L. (2004). Kinematic differentiation of prosodic categories in normal and disordered language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(5), 1088–1102. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/081)
  • Goffman, L., Factor, L., Barna, M., Cai, F., & Feld, I. (2023). Phonological and articulatory deficits in the production of novel signs in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 66(3), 1051–1067. https://doi.org/10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00434
  • Goffman, L., & Gerken, L. A. (2020). An alternative to the procedural∼declarative memory account of developmental language disorder. Journal of Communication Disorders, 83), doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.105946
  • Goffman, L., Gerken, L. A., & Lucchesi, J. (2007). Relations between segmental and motor variability in prosodically complex non-word sequences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(2), 444–458. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/031)
  • Goffman, L., Heisler, L., & Chakraborty, R. (2006). Mapping of prosodic structure onto words and phrases in children’s and adults’ speech production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(1-3), 25–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960400001820
  • Goffman, L., & Smith, A. (1999). Development and phonetic differentiation of speech movement patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(3), 649–660. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.25.3.649
  • Goffman, L., & Westover, S. (2013). Interactivity in prosodic representations in children. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 1032–1056. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000505
  • Goldowsky, B. N., & Newport, E. (1993). Modeling the effects of processing limitations on the acquisition of morphology: the less is more hypothesis. In E. Clark (Ed.), The proceedings of the 24th annual child language research forum (pp. 124–139). CSLI.
  • Gopnik, M. (1997). Language deficits and genetic factors. Trends in Cognitive Science, 1, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01005-X
  • Heisler, L., Goffman, L., & Younger, B. (2010). Lexical and articulatory interactions in children’s language production. Developmental Science, 13(5), 722–730. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00930.x
  • Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: a review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(2), 149–171. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820010019874
  • Hsu, J., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Sequence-specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17(3), 352–365. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12125
  • Hudson Kam, C. L., & Newport, E. L. (2005). Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development, 1(2), 151–195.
  • Kam, H., Newport, C. L., & L, E. (2009). Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages. Cognitive Psychology, 59(1), 30–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.01.001
  • Kemler Nelson, D., Jusczyk, P. W., Mandel, D. R., Myers, J., Turk, A. E., & Gerken, L. A. (1995). The headturn preference procedure for testing auditory perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 111–116.
  • Kreidler, K., Vuolo, J., & Goffman, L. (2023). Manual rhythmic grouping in children with developmental language disorder. epub ahead of print.
  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior; the Hixon Symposium (pp. 112–146). Wiley.
  • Leonard, L. B. (2014a). Children with specific language impairment. MIT press.
  • Leonard, L. B. (2014b). Specific language impairment across languages. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12053
  • Leonard, L. B., & Deevy, P. (2020). Retrieval practice and word learning in children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(10), 3252–3262. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00006.
  • Levelt, W. J. M., & Wheeldon, L. (1994). Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? Cognition, 50(1-3), 239–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90030-2
  • Lind, J., Vinken, V., Jonsson, M., Ghirlanda, S., & Enquist, M.. (2023). A test of memory for stimulus sequences in great apes. PLoS ONE, 18(9), e0290546. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0290546
  • Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Angela, T., Morgan, A. T., & Ullman, M. T. (2014). Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51 (100), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.011
  • McGregor, K. K., Arbisi-Kelm, T., Eden, N., & Oleson, J. (2020). The word learning profile of adults with developmental language disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 5, 239694151989931–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941519899311
  • Montgomery, J., Gillam, R., & Evans, J. (2021). A new memory perspective on the sentence comprehension deficits of school-age children with developmental language disorder: Implications for theory, assessment, and intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 52(2), 449–466. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_LSHSS-20-00128
  • Moreton, E. (2008). Analytic bias and phonological typology. Phonology, 25(1), 83–127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675708001413.
  • Moreton, E. (2012). Inter- and intra-dimensional dependencies in implicit phonotactic learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(1), 165–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.12.003
  • Moreton, E., & Pater, J. (2012). Structure and substance in artificial-phonology learning, part II: Substance. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(11), 702–718. https://doi.org/10.1002/lnc3.366
  • Moreton, E., Pater, J., & Pertsova, K. (2015). Phonological concept learning. Cognitive Science, 40(1), 1–66.
  • Moreton, E., & Pertsova, K. (2014). Pastry phonotactics: Is phonological learning special? In H.-L. Huang, E. Poole, & A. Rysling (Eds.), Proceedings of the 43rd annual meeting of the northeast linguistic society (pp. 1–14). Graduate Linguistics Students’ Association.
  • Onishi, K. H., Chambers, K. E., & Fisher, C. (2002). Learning phonotactic constraints from brief auditory experience. Cognition, 83(1), B13–B23. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00165-2
  • Quémart, P., & Maillart, C. (2016). The sensitivity of children with SLI to phonotactic probabilities during lexical access. Journal of Communication Disorders, 61, 48–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.03.005
  • Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38(4), 850–863. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3804.850
  • Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1239–1257. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1239
  • Richtsmeier, P. T., & Goffman, L. (2015). Learning trajectories for speech motor performance in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 55, 31–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.02.001
  • Sack, L., Dollaghan, C., & Goffman, L. (2022). Contributions of early motor deficits in predicting language outcomes among preschoolers with developmental language disorder. International Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 24(4), 362–374. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549507.2021.1998629
  • Saffran, J. R., & Thiessen, E. D. (2003). Pattern induction by infant language learners. Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 484–494. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.484
  • Saletta, M., Goffman, L., Ward, C., & Oleson, J. (2018). Influence of language load on speech motor skill in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(3), 675–689. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0066
  • Sanjeevan, T., & Mainela-Arnold, E. (2017). Procedural motor learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 3259–3269. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0457
  • Sanjeevan, T., & Mainela-Arnold, E. (2019). Characterizing the motor skills in children with specific language impairment. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 71(1), 42–55. https://doi.org/10.1159/000493262
  • Scoppa, M., Gerken, L. A., Glickman, S., Benham, S., & Goffman, L. (2022). The acquisition of a rule-based sound sequence in typically developing 4- to 6-year-old children. Paper presented at the Poster presented at The Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI.
  • Shepard, R. N., Hovland, C. I., & Jenkins, H. M. (1961). Learning and memorization of classifications. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 75(13). https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093825
  • Smalle, E. H. M., Muylle, M., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W. (2017). The different time course of phonotactic constraint learning in children and adults: Evidence from speech errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(11), 1821–1827. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000405.
  • Smith, A., & Goffman, L. (1998). Stability and patterning of speech movement sequences in children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(1), 18–30. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4101.18
  • Smith, A., Goffman, L., Zelaznik, H., Ying, G., & McGillem, C. (1995). Spatiotemporal stability and patterning of speech movement sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 104(3), 493–501. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00231983
  • Smith, A., Johnson, M., McGillem, C., & Goffman, L. (2000). On the assessment of stability and patterning of speech movements. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(1), 277–286. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4301.277
  • Smith, A., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2004). Development of functional synergies for speech motor coordination in childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology, 45(1), 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20009
  • Smith, J. D., Berg, M. E., Cook, R. G., Murphy, M. S., Crossley, M. J., Boomer, J., Spiering, B., Beran, M. J., Church, B. A., Ashby, F. G., & Grace, R. C. (2012). Implicit and explicit categorization: A tale of four species. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2355–2369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.003
  • Spencer, R. M. C., Zelaznik, H. N., Diedrichsen, J., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science, 300(5624), 1437–1439. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1083661
  • Ullman, M. T., Earle, F. S., Walenski, M., & Janacsek, K. (2020). The neurocognition of developmental disorders of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 389–417. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011555
  • Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41(3), 399–433. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70276-4
  • Ullman, M. T., & Pullman, M. Y. (2015). A compensatory role for declarative memory in neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 205–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.008
  • Veit, L., Tian, L. Y., Monroy Hernandez, C. J., & Brainard, M. S. (2021). Songbirds can learn flexible contextual control over syllable sequencing. Elife, 1(10), e61610. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.61610.
  • Vuolo, J., & Goffman, L. (2018). Language skill mediates the relationship between language load and articulatory variability in children with language and speech sound disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(12), 3010–3022. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0055
  • Vuolo, J., & Goffman, L. (2020). Vowel accuracy and segmental variability differentiate children with developmental language disorder in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(12), 3945–3960. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00166
  • Vuolo, J., Goffman, L., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2017). Deficits in coordinative bimanual timing precision in children With specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(2), 393–405. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0100
  • Walsh, B., & Smith, A. (2002). Articulatory movements in adolescents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1119–1133. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/090)
  • Warker, J. A. (2013). Investigating the retention and time course of phonotactic constraint learning from production experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 96–109.
  • Warker, J. A., & Dell, G. S. (2006). Speech errors reflect newly learned phonotactic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), 387–398. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.387
  • Zelaznik, H. N., & Goffman, L. (2010). Generalized motor abilities and timing behavior in children With specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 383–393