463
Views
20
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Papers

Theory of mind and use of cognitive state terms by adolescents with traumatic brain injuryFootnote

&
Pages 1054-1070 | Received 23 Jun 2007, Accepted 14 Aug 2007, Published online: 22 Oct 2010

REFERENCES

  • Adolphs , R. 1999 . Social cognition and the human brain. . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 3 : 469 – 479 .
  • Adrian , J. E. , Clemente , R. A. , Villanueva , L. and Rieffe , C. 2005 . Parent–child picture‐book reading, mothers' mental state language and children's theory of mind. . Journal of Child Language , 32 (3) : 673 – 686 .
  • Apperly , I. A. , Samson , D. , Chiavarino , C. and Humphreys , G. W. 2004 . Frontal and temporo‐parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: Neuropsychological evidence from a false‐belief task with reduced language and executive demands. . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 16 (10) : 1773 – 1784 .
  • Baron‐Cohen , S. , Ring , H. A. , Bullmore , E. T. , Wheelwright , S. , Ashwin , C. and Williams , S. C. R. 2000 . The amygdala theory of autism. . Neuroscience and Behavioural Reviews , 24 : 355 – 364 .
  • Barrett , L. , Henzi , P. and Rendall , D. 2007 . Social brains, simple minds: Does social complexity really require cognitive complexity? . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B , 362 (1480) : 561 – 575 .
  • Berndt , T. J. 1979 . Developmental changes in conformity to peers and parents. . Developmental Psychology , 15 : 608 – 616 .
  • Berndt , T. J. and Hoyle , S. G. 1985 . Stability and change in childhood and adolescent friendships. . Developmental Psychology , 21 : 1007 – 1015 .
  • Bibby , H. and McDonald , S. 2005 . Theory of mind after traumatic brain injury. . Neuropsychologia , 43 (1) : 99 – 114 .
  • Bird , C. M. , Castelli , F. , Malik , O. , Frith , U. and Husain , M. 2004 . The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on ‘Theory of Mind’ and cognition. . Brain , 127 (Pt 4) : 914 – 928 .
  • Bosacki , S. L. 2003 . Psychological pragmatics in preadolescents: Sociomoral understanding, self‐worth, and school behaviour. . Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 32 (2) : 141 – 155 .
  • Brookshire , B. , Levin , H. S. , Song , J. and Zhang , L. 2004 . Components of executive function in typically developing and head‐injured children. . Developmental Neuropsychology , 25 (1–2) : 61 – 83 .
  • Brune , M. and Brune‐Cohrs , U. 2006 . Theory of mind—evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. . Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews , 30 (4) : 437 – 455 .
  • Channon , S. , Pellijeff , A. and Rule , A. 2005 . Social cognition after head injury: Sarcasm and theory of mind. . Brain and Language , 93 (2) : 123 – 134 .
  • Chapman , S. B. , Levin , H. S. , Wanek , A. , Weyauch , J. and Kufera , J. 1998 . Discourse after closed head injury in young children. . Brain and Language , 61 (3) : 395 – 419 .
  • Chapman , S. B. , Sparks , G. , Levin , H. S. , Dennis , M. , Roncadin , C. Zhang , L. 2004 . Discourse macrolevel processing after severe pediatric traumatic brain injury. . Developmental Neuropsychology , 25 (1–2) : 37 – 60 .
  • Clegg , J. , Hollis , C. , Mawhood , L. and Rutter , M. 2005 . Developmental language disorders – a follow‐up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. . Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 46 (2) : 128 – 149 .
  • Coelho , C. , Ylvisaker , M. and Turkstra , L. S. 2005 . Nonstandardized assessment approaches for individuals with traumatic brain injuries. . Seminars in Speech and Language , 26 (4) : 223 – 241 .
  • Colle , L. , Baron‐Cohen , S. and Hill , J. 2007 . Do children with autism have a theory of mind? A non‐verbal test of autism vs. specific language impairment. . Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 37 (4) : 716 – 723 .
  • Critchley , H. D. , Daly , E. M. , Bullmore , E. T. , Williams , S. C. R. , Van Amelsvoort , T. Robertson , D. M. 2000a . The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: Changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. . Brain , 123 : 2203 – 2212 .
  • Critchley , H. D. , Daly , E. M. , Phillips , M. , Brammer , M. , Bullmore , E. Williams , S. 2000b . Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. . Human Brain Mapping , 9 : 93 – 105 .
  • Dennis , M. and Barnes , M. A. 1990 . Knowing the meaning, getting the point, bridging the gap, and carrying the message: Aspects of discourse following closed head injury in childhood and adolescence. . Brain and Language , 39 : 428 – 446 .
  • Englund , M. M. , Levy , A. K. , Hyson , D. M. and Sroufe , L. A. 2000 . Adolescent social competence: Effectiveness in a group setting. . Child Development , 71 (4) : 1049 – 1060 .
  • Ewing‐Cobbs , L. , Fletcher , J. , Levin , H. S. and Landry , S. H. 1985 . “ Language disorders after pediatric closed head injury. ” . In Language and neurology , Edited by: Darby , J . New York : Academic Press .
  • Ewing‐Cobbs , L. , Levin , H. S. , Eisenberg , H. M. and Fletcher , J. M. 1987 . Language functions following closed‐head injury in children and adolescents. . Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology , 9 (5) : 575 – 592 .
  • Galski , T. , Tompkins , C. and Johnston , M. V. 1998 . Competence in discourse as a measure of social integration and quality of life in persons with traumatic brain injury. . Brain Injury , 12 (9) : 769 – 782 .
  • Garrett , A. S. , Menon , V. , MacKenzie , K. and Reiss , A. L. 2004 . Here's looking at you, kid: Neural systems underlying face and gaze processing in fragile X syndrome. . Archives of General Psychiatry , 61 (3) : 281 – 288 .
  • Gillot , A. , Furniss , F. and Walter , A. 2004 . ToM ability in children with specific language ability. . Child Language Teaching and Therapy , 20 : 1 – 11 .
  • Hanten , G. , Bartha , M. and Levin , H. S. 2000 . Metacognition following pediatric traumatic brain injury: a preliminary study. . Developmental Neuropsychology , 18 (3) : 383 – 398 .
  • Happé , F. , Brownell , H. and Winner , E. 1999 . Acquired ‘theory of mind’ impairments following stroke. . Cognition , 70 : 211 – 240 .
  • Hartley , L. L. 1995 . Cognitive‐communicative abilities following brain injury: A functional approach , San Diego, CA : Singular Publishing Group, Inc .
  • Havet‐Thomassin , V. , Allain , P. , Etcharry‐Bouyx , F. and Le Gall , D. 2006 . What about theory of mind after severe brain injury? . Brain Injury , 20 (1) : 83 – 91 .
  • Hollingshead , A. B. 1975 . Four factor index of social status , New Haven, CT : Yale University .
  • Janusz , J. A. , Kirkwood , M. W. , Yeates , K. O. and Taylor , H. G. 2002 . Social problem‐solving skills in children with traumatic brain injury: Long‐term outcomes and prediction of social competence. . Neuropsychological Development Cognition Section C Child Neuropsychology , 8 (3) : 179 – 194 .
  • Johnson , P. , Thomas‐Stonell , N. , Rumney , P. and Oddson , B. 2006 . Long‐term outcomes of pediatric acquired brain injury. . Brain and Cognition , 60 (2) : 205 – 206 .
  • Johnston , J. R. , Miller , J. and Tallal , P. 2001 . Use of cognitive state predicates by language‐impaired children. . International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders , 36 (3) : 349 – 370 .
  • Keenan , H. T. and Bratton , S. L. 2006 . Epidemiology and outcomes of pediatric traumatic brain injury. . Developmental Neuroscience , 28 (4–5) : 256 – 263 .
  • Kennedy , M. R. T. and Yorkston , K. M. 2000 . Accuracy of metamemory after traumatic brain injury: Predictions during verbal learning. . Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research , 43 : 1072 – 1086 .
  • Levin , H. S. , Zhang , L. , Dennis , M. , Ewing‐Cobbs , L. , Schachar , R. Max , J. 2004 . Psychosocial outcome of TBI in children with unilateral frontal lesions. . Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society , 10 (3) : 305 – 316 .
  • Martin , I. and McDonald , S. 2003 . Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders. . Brain and Language , 85 : 451 – 466 .
  • Martin , I. and McDonald , S. 2005 . Evaluating the causes of impaired irony comprehension following traumatic brain injury. . Aphasiology , 19 (8) : 712 – 730 .
  • McDonald , S. 1993a . Pragmatic language skills after closed head injury: Ability to meet the informational needs of the listener. . Brain and Language , 44 : 28 – 46 .
  • McDonald , S. 1993b . Viewing the brain sideways? Frontal versus right hemisphere explanations of nonaphasic language disorders. . Aphasiology , 7 (6) : 535 – 549 .
  • McDonald , S. 2000 . Putting communication disorders in context after traumatic brain injury. . Aphasiology , 14 (4) : 339 – 347 .
  • Miller , C. 2004 . False belief and sentence complement performance in children with specific language impairment. . International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders , 39 (2) : 191 – 213 .
  • Miller , C. A. 2006 . Developmental relationships between language and theory of mind. . American Journal of Speech Language Pathology , 15 (2) : 142 – 154 .
  • Miller , J. F. and Chapman , J. 1994–2004 . The SALT guide (Standard Version 8) , Madison, WI : Language Analysis Laboratory, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin .
  • Moll , J. , Zahn , R. , de Oliveira‐Souza , R. , Krueger , F. and Grafman , J. 2005 . Opinion: The neural basis of human moral cognition. . Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 6 (10) : 799 – 809 .
  • Mundy , P. and Markus , J. 1997 . On the nature of communication and language impairment in autism. . Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities: Research Reviews , 3 : 343 – 349 .
  • Mutter , B. , Alcorn , M. B. and Welsh , M. 2006 . Theory of mind and executive function: Working‐memory capacity and inhibitory control as predictors of false‐belief task performance. . Perceptual and Motor Skills , 102 (3) : 819 – 835 .
  • Onishi , K. H. , Baillargeon , R. and Leslie , A. M. 2007 . 15‐month‐old infants detect violations in pretend scenarios. . Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam) , 124 (1) : 106 – 128 .
  • Rowe , A. D. , Bullock , P. R. , Pokley , C. E. and Morris , R. G. 2001 . ‘Theory of mind’ impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. . Brain , 124 : 600 – 616 .
  • Sabbagh , M. A. , Xu , F. , Carlson , S. M. , Moses , L. J. and Lee , K. 2006 . The development of executive functioning and theory of mind. A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. . Psychological Science , 17 (1) : 74 – 81 .
  • Sabers , D. L. 1996 . By their tests we will know them. . Language Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools , 27 (2) : 102 – 108 .
  • Schultz , J. , Imamizu , H. , Kawato , M. and Frith , C. D. 2004 . Activation of the human superior temporal gyrus during observation of goal attribution by intentional objects. . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 16 (10) : 1695 – 1705 .
  • Shamay‐Tsoory , S. G. , Tomer , R. and Aharon‐Peretz , J. 2005 . The neuroanatomical basis of understanding sarcasm and its relationship to social cognition. . Neuropsychology , 19 (3) : 288 – 300 .
  • Silliman , E. R. , Diehl , S. F. , Huntley Bahr , R. , Hnath‐Chisolm , T. , Zenko , C. B. and Friedman , S. A. 2003 . A new look at performance on theory‐of‐mind tasks by adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. . Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools , 34 : 236 – 252 .
  • SPSS . 2005 . SPSS statistical analysis software (Version 11.0 for OSX) , Chicago, , USA : SPSS, Inc .
  • Stone , V. E. , Baron‐Cohen , S. and Knight , R. T. 1998 . Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 10 (5) : 640 – 656 .
  • Stuss , D. T. , Gallup , G. G. and Alexander , M. P. 2001 . The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. . Brain , 124 : 279 – 286 .
  • Tager‐Flusberg , H. 1999 . A psychological approach to understanding the social and language impairments in autism. . International Reviews in Psychiatry , 11 (4) : 325 – 334 .
  • Togher , L. , Hand , L. and Code , C. 1996 . A new perspective on the relationship between communication impairment and disempowerment following head injury in information exchanges. . Disability and Rehabilitation , 18 (11) : 559 – 566 .
  • Turkstra , L. S. 2000 . Should my shirt be tucked in or left out? The communication context of adolescence. . Aphasiology , 14 (4) : 349 – 364 .
  • Turkstra , L. S. , Dixon , T. M. and Baker , K. K. 2004 . Theory of mind and social beliefs in adolescents with traumatic brain injury. . NeuroRehabilitation , 19 (3) : 245 – 256 .
  • Turkstra , L. S. and Holland , A. L. 1998 . Assessment of syntax after adolescent brain injury: Effects of memory on test performance. . Journal of Speech Language Hearing Research , 41 (1) : 137 – 149 .
  • Turkstra , L. S. , McDonald , S. and DePompei , R. 2001 . Social information processing in adolescents: Data from normally developing adolescents and preliminary data from their peers with traumatic brain injury. . Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation , 16 (5) : 469 – 483 .
  • Turkstra , L. S. , McDonald , S. and Kaufmann , P. M. 1996 . Assessment of pragmatic communication skills in adolescents after traumatic brain injury. . Brain Injury , 10 (5) : 329 – 345 .
  • Van Leer , E. and Turkstra , L. 1999 . The effect of elicitation task on discourse coherence and cohesion in adolescents with brain injury. . Journal of Communication Disorders , 32 (5) : 327 – 348; quiz 348–329 .
  • Watts , A. J. and Douglas , J. M. 2006 . Interpreting facial expression and communication competence following severe traumatic brain injury. . Aphasiology , 20 (8) : 707 – 722 .
  • Wiseman‐Hakes , C. , Stewart , M. L. , Wasserman , R. and Schuller , R. 1998 . Peer group training of pragmatic skills in adolescents with acquired brain injury. . Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation , 13 (6) : 23 – 38 .
  • Yeates , K. O. , Swift , E. , Taylor , H. G. , Wade , S. L. , Drotar , D. Stancin , T. 2004 . Short‐ and long‐term social outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury. . Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society , 10 (3) : 412 – 426 .
  • Zanini , S. , Bryan , K. , DeLuca , G. and Bava , A. 2005 . The effects of age and education on pragmatic features of verbal communication: Evidence from the Italian version of the Right Hemisphere Language Battery (I‐RHLB). . Aphasiology , 19 (12) : 1107 – 1133 .

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.