703
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Phonological facilitation effects on naming latencies and viewing times during noun and verb naming in agrammatic and anomic aphasia

&
Pages 1164-1188 | Received 14 Oct 2014, Accepted 23 Mar 2015, Published online: 27 Apr 2015

References

  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database [CD-ROM]. In Linguistic Data Consortium. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-7. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
  • Bates, E., Chen, S., Tzeng, O., Li, P., & Opie, M. (1991). The noun-verb problem in Chinese aphasia. Brain and Language, 41, 203–233. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(91)90153-R
  • Baum, S. R. (1997). Phonological, semantic, and mediated priming in aphasia. Brain and Language, 60, 347–359. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1829
  • Berndt, R. S., Haendiges, A. N., Mitchum, C. C., & Sandson, J. (1997b). Verb retrieval in aphasia. 2. Relationship to sentence processing. Brain and Language, 56, 107–137. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1728
  • Berndt, R. S., Mitchum, C. C., Haendiges, A. N., & Sandson, J. (1997a). Verb retrieval in aphasia 1. Characterizing single word impairments. Brain and Language, 56, 68–106. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1727
  • Bock, K., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 945–984). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2009). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [computer program]. Retrieved from www.praat.org
  • Butterworth, B. (1980). Some constraints on models of language production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production, Vol. 1, Speech and talk. London: Academic Press.
  • Caramazza, A., & Hillis, A. E. (1990). Where do semantic errors come from? Cortex, 26, 95–122. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80077-9
  • Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., & Villa, G. (1986). The role of the (output) phonological buffer in reading, writing, and repetition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 37–76. doi:10.1080/02643298608252669
  • Caramazza, A., Papagno, C., & Ruml, W. (2000). The selective impairment of phonological processing in speech production. Brain and Language, 75, 428–450. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2379
  • Chen, S., & Bates, E. (1998). The dissociation between nouns and verbs in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia: Findings from Chinese. Aphasiology, 12, 5–36. doi:10.1080/02687039808249441
  • Cho-Reyes, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2012). Verb and sentence production and comprehension in aphasia: Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS). Aphasiology, 26, 1250–1277. doi:10.1080/02687038.2012.693584
  • Columbo, L. (1986). Activation and inhibition with orthographically similar words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 226–234.
  • Cuetos, F., Aguado, G., & Caramazza, A. (2000). Dissociation of semantic and phonological errors in naming. Brain and Language, 75, 451–460. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2383
  • Damian, M. F., & Martin, R. C. (1999). Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 345–361.
  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
  • Dell, G. S., & O’Seaghdha, P. G. (1992). Stages of lexical access in language production. Cognition, 42, 287–314. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90046-K
  • Dell, G. S., & Reich, P. A. (1981). Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 611–629. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90202-4
  • Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104, 801–838. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.801
  • Foygel, D., & Dell, G. S. (2000). Models of impaired lexical access in speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 182–216. doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2716
  • Gainotti, G., Silveri, M., Villa, G., & Miceli, G. (1986). Anomia with and without lexical comprehension disorders. Brain and Language, 29, 18–33. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(86)90031-3
  • Gordon, J. K., & Baum, S. R. (1994). Rhyme priming in aphasia: The role of phonology in lexical access. Brain and Language, 47, 661–683. doi:10.1006/brln.1994.1062
  • Griffin, Z. M. (2001). Gaze durations during speech reflect word selection and phonological encoding. Cognition, 82, B1–B14. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00138-X
  • Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (2000). What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274–279. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00255
  • Hashimoto, N., & Thompson, C. K. (2009). The use of the picture-word interference paradigm to examine naming abilities in aphasic individuals. Apha Siology, 24, 580–611.
  • Hillis, A. E., Boatman, D., Hart, J., & Gordon, B. (1999). Making sense out of jargon: A neurolinguistic and computational account of jargon aphasia. Neurology, 53, 1813–1813. doi:10.1212/WNL.53.8.1813
  • Hillis, A. E., Rapp, B., Romani, C., & Caramazza, A. (1990). Selective impairment of semantics in lexical processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 191–243. doi:10.1080/02643299008253442
  • Hyun, J. 2003. Retrieval of nouns and verbs in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Korean Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, [in Korean] 8, 171–187.
  • Jonkers, R., & Bastiaanse, R. (1998). How selective are selective word class deficits? Two case studies of action and object naming. Aphasiology, 12, 245–256. doi:10.1080/02687039808249453
  • Kay, J., & Ellis, A. (1987). A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia. Brain, 110, 613–629. doi:10.1093/brain/110.3.613
  • Kempen, G., & Hujibers, P. (1983). The lexicalization process in sentence production and naming: Indirect election of words. Cognition, 14, 185–209. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90029-X
  • Kertesz, A. (2006). Western Aphasia Battery – Revised. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp.
  • Kim, M., & Thompson, C. K. (2000). Patterns of comprehension and production of nouns and verbs in agrammatism: Implications for lexical organization. Brain and Language, 74, 1–25. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2315
  • Kohn, S. E., Lorch, P., & Pearson, D. M. (1989). Verb finding in aphasia. Cortex, 25, 57–69. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(89)80006-1
  • Laine, M., & Martin, N. (1996). Lexical retrieval deficit in picture naming: Implications for word production models. Brain and Language, 53, 283–314. doi:10.1006/brln.1996.0050
  • Lambon Ralph, M. A., Sage, K., & Roberts, J. (2000). Classical anomia: A neuropsychological perspective on speech production. Neuropsychologia, 38, 186–202. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00056-1
  • Lee, J., Mack, J., & Thompson, C. K. (2012). Verbal morphology in agrammatic and anomic aphasia: Comparison of structured vs. narrative elicitation tasks. Poster presented at Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Lake Tahoe, CA.
  • Lee, J., Milman, L. H., & Thompson, C. K. (2008). Functional category production in English agrammatism. Aphasiology, 22, 893–905. doi:10.1080/02687030701865670
  • Lee, J., & Thompson, C. K. (2011a). Real-time production of arguments and adjuncts in normal and agrammatic speakers. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 985–1021. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.496237
  • Lee, J., & Thompson, C. K. (2011b). Real-time production of unergative and unaccusative sentences in normal and agrammatic speakers: An eyetracking study. Aphasiology, 25, 813–825. doi:10.1080/02687038.2010.542563
  • Lee, J., & Thompson, C. K. (experimental version). Northwestern Assessment of Verb Inflection.
  • Lee, J., Yoshida, M., & Thompson, C. K. (accepted). Grammatical planning units during real-time sentence production in agrammatic aphasia and healthy speakers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
  • Levelt, W. J. M. (2001). Spoken word production: A theory of lexical access. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 13464–13471. doi:10.1073/pnas.231459498
  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99001776
  • Levelt, W. J. M., Schriefers, H., Vorberb, D., Meyer, A. S., Pechmann, T., & Havinga, J. (1991). The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming. Psychological Review, 98, 122–142. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.1.122
  • Lupker, S. J., & Columbo, L. (1994). Inhibitory effects in form priming: Evaluating a phonological competition explanation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 437–451.
  • Luzzatti, C., & Chierchia, G. (2002). On the nature of selective deficits involving nouns and verbs. Revista Di Linguistica, 14.1, 43–71.
  • Mack, J. E., Cho-Reyes, S., Kloet, J. D., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, -M.-M., & Thompson, C. K. (2013). Phonological facilitation of object naming in agrammatic and logopenic primary progressive aphasia (PPA). Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30, 172–193. doi:10.1080/02643294.2013.835717
  • Martin, N., Gagnon, D. A., Schwartz, M. F., Dell, G. S., & Saffran, E. M. (1996). Phonological facilitation of semantic errors in normal and aphasic speakers. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 257–282. doi:10.1080/016909696387187
  • McCarthy, R., & Warrington, E. K. (1985). Category specificity in an agrammatic patient: The relative impairment of verb retrieval and comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 23, 709–727. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(85)90079-X
  • Meyer, A. S., & Schriefers, H. (1991). Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effect of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 1146–1160.
  • Meyer, A. S., Sleiderink, A. M., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1998). Viewing and naming objects: Eye movements during noun phrase production. Cognition, 66, B25–B33. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00009-2
  • Meyer, A. S., & van der Meulen, F. F. (2000). Phonological priming effects on speech onset latencies and viewing times in object naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 314–319. doi:10.3758/BF03212987
  • Miceli, G., Silveri, M. C., Villa, G., & Caramazza, A. (1984). On the basis for the agrammatic’s difficulty in producing main verbs. Cortex, 20, 207–220. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(84)80038-6
  • Milberg, W., Blumstein, S., & Dworetzky, B. (1988). Phonological processing and lexical access in aphasia. Brain and Language, 34, 279–293. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(88)90139-3
  • Myerson, R., & Goodglass, H. (1972). Transformational grammar of three agrammatic patients. Language and Speech, 15, 40–50.
  • O’Seagdha, P. G., & Marin, J. W. (2000). Phonological competition and cooperation in form-related priming: Sequential and nonsequential processes in word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 57–73.
  • Odekar, A., Hallowell, B., Kruse, H., Moates, D., & Lee, C.-Y. (2009). Validity of eye movement methods and indices for capturing semantic (associative) priming effects. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, 52, 31–48.
  • Rapp, B., & Goldrick, M. (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review, 107, 460–499. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.460
  • Rochon, E., Laird, L., Bose, A., & Scofield, J. (2005). Mapping therapy for sentence production impairments in nonfluent aphasia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15, 1–36. doi:10.1080/09602010343000327
  • Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1996). Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment of Starreveld and LaHeij (1995). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 246–251.
  • Saffran, E., Schwartz, M., & Marin, O. S. M. (1980). Evidence from aphasia: Isolating the components of a production model. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production Vol. 1: Speech and Talk. New York: Academic Press.
  • Schriefers, H., Meyer, A., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1990). Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 86–102. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N
  • Schwartz, M. F., Dell, G. S., Martin, N., Gahl, S., & Sobel, P. (2006). A case-series test of the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Evidence from picture naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 228–264. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.10.001
  • Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., Fink, R. B., Myers, J. L., & Martin, N. (1994). Mapping therapy: A treatment programme for agrammatism. Aphasiology, 8, 19–54. doi:10.1080/02687039408248639
  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.
  • Starreveld, P. A. (2000). On the interpretation of onsets of auditory context effects in word production. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 497–525. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2693
  • Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W. (1996). Time-course analyses of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 896–918.
  • Stemberger, J. P. (1985). An interactive activation model of language production. In A. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language (pp. 143–186). London: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
  • Thompson, C. K. (2011). Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University. Retrieved from http://flintbox.com/public/project/9299/
  • Thompson, C. K., Shapiro, L. P., Tait, M., Jacobs, B., Schneider, S. L., & Ballard, K. (1995). A system for the linguistic analysis of agrammatic language production. Brain and Language, 51, 124–129.
  • Williams, S. E., & Canter, G. J. (1987). Action-naming performance in four syndromes of aphasia. Brain and Language, 32, 124–136. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(87)90120-9
  • Wilshire, C. E., Keall, L. M., Stuart, E. J., & O’Donnell, D. J. (2007). Exploring the dynamics of aphasic word production using the picture-word interference task: A case study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 939–953. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.026
  • Wilshire, C. E., & McCarthy, R. A. (1996). Experimental investigations of an impairment in phonological encoding. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 1059–1098. doi:10.1080/026432996381782
  • Wilshire, C. E., & Saffran, E. M. (2005). Contrasting effects of phonological priming in aphasic word production. Cognition, 95, 31–71. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.004
  • Zingeser, L. B., & Berndt, R. S. (1988). Grammatical class and context effects in a case of pure anomia: Implications for models of language production. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 473–516. doi:10.1080/02643298808253270
  • Zingeser, L. B., & Berndt, R. S. (1990). Retrieval of nouns and verbs in agrammatism and anomia. Brain and Language, 39, 14–32. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(90)90002-X

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.