491
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The implicit cognition of reciprocal exchange: automatic retrieval of positive and negative experiences with partners in a prisoner's dilemma game

, , &
Pages 657-670 | Received 06 Mar 2015, Accepted 22 Jan 2016, Published online: 02 Mar 2016

References

  • Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396. doi: 10.1126/science.7466396
  • Barclay, P. (2008). Enhanced recognition of defectors depends on their rarity. Cognition, 107, 817–828. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.013
  • Barclay, P., & Lalumière, M. L. (2006). Do people differentially remember cheaters? Human Nature, 17, 98–113. doi: 10.1007/s12110-006-1022-y
  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Bell, R., & Buchner, A. (2012). How adaptive is memory for cheaters? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 403–408. doi: 10.1177/0963721412458525
  • Bell, R., Buchner, A., & Musch, J. (2010). Enhanced old-new recognition and source memory for faces of cooperators and defectors in a social-dilemma game. Cognition, 117, 261–275. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.020
  • Bell, R., Buchner, A., Kroneisen, M., & Giang, T. (2012). On the flexibility of social source memory: A test of the emotional incongruity hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1512–1529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028219
  • Bell, R., Sasse, J., Möller, M., Czernochowski, D., Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2016). Event-related potentials in response to cheating and cooperation in a social dilemma game. Psychophysiology, 53, 216–228. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12561
  • Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond fear: Rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychological Science, 19, 362–370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x 18399889 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x
  • Buchner, A., Bell, R., Mehl, B., & Musch, J. (2009). No enhanced recognition memory, but better source memory for faces of cheaters. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 212–224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.01.004
  • Carlston, D. E., Skowronski, J. J., & Sparks, C. (1995). Savings in relearning: II. On the formation of behavior-based trait associations and inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 420–436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.429
  • Clark, K., & Sefton, M. (2001). The sequential prisoner's dilemma: Evidence on reciprocation. The Economic Journal, 111, 51–68. doi: 10.1111/1468-0297.00588
  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1989). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture: II. Case study: A computational theory of social exchange. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10, 51–97. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(89)90013-7
  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–228). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2005). Neurocognitive adaptations designed for social exchange. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 584–627). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
  • Delton, A. W., Nemirow, J., Robertson, T. E., Cimino, A., & Cosmides, L. (2013). Merely opting out of a public good is moralized: An error management approach to cooperation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 621–638. doi:10.1037/a0033495
  • Dugatkin, L. A. (2002). Animal cooperation among unrelated individuals. Naturwissenschaften, 89, 533–541.
  • Engell, A. D., Todorov, A., & Haxby, J. V. (2010). Common neural mechanisms for the evaluation of facial trustworthiness and emotional expressions as revealed by behavioral adaptation. Perception, 39, 931–941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p6633 20842970 doi: 10.1068/p6633
  • Ermer, E., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Cheater detection mechanism. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Social Psychology (pp. 138–140). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225 12172003 doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225
  • Fischbacher, U., Gächter, S., & Fehr, E. (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71, 397–404. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1765(01)00394-9
  • Giang, T., Bell, R., & Buchner, A. (2012). Does facial resemblance enhance cooperation? PLoS ONE, 7, e47809. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047809
  • Hruschka, D. J., & Henrich, J. (2006). Friendship, cliquishness, and the emergence of cooperation. Journal of theoretical biology, 239, 1–15. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.07.006
  • Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Chance, S. (2002). Decisions and the evolution of memory: Multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychological Review, 109, 306–329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.2.306
  • Koranyi, N., Schreckenbach, F., & Rothermund, K. (2015). The implicit cognition of lying: Knowledge about having lied to a question is retrieved automatically. Social Cognition, 33, 67–84. doi: 10.1521/soco.2015.33.1.67
  • Kroneisen, M., Woehe, L., & Rausch, L. S. (2015). Expectancy effects in source memory: How moving to a bad neighborhood can change your memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 179–189. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0655-9.
  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces – KDEF. Stockholm, Sweden: Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet. ISBN 91-630-7164-9.
  • Mealey, L., Daood, C., & Krage, M. (1996). Enhanced memory for faces of cheaters. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 119–128. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(95)00131-X
  • Mehl, B., & Buchner, A. (2008). No enhanced memory for faces of cheaters. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.001
  • Milinski, M., & Wedekind, C. (1998). Working memory constrains human cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 13755–13758. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.23.13755
  • Minear, M., & Park, D. C. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36, 630–633. doi: 10.3758/BF03206543
  • Mussel, P., Goritz, A. S., & Hewig, J. (2013). The value of a smile: Facial expression affects ultimatum-game responses. Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 381–385.
  • Müller, S., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (in press). Relevance drives attention: Attentional bias for gain-and loss-related stimuli is driven by delayed disengagement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.
  • Oda, R. (1997). Biased face recognition in the prisoner's dilemma game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 309–315. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00014-7
  • Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380
  • Rothermund, K. (2011). Counter-regulation and control-dependency: Affective processing biases in the service of action regulation. Social Psychology, 42, 56–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000043
  • Rothermund, K., Voss, A., & Wentura, D. (2008). Counter-regulation in affective attentional biases: A basic mechanism that warrants flexibility in emotion and motivation. Emotion, 8, 34–46. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.1.34
  • Rothermund, K., Wentura, D., & Bak, P. M. (2001). Automatic attention to stimuli signalling chances and dangers: Moderating effects of positive and negative goal and action contexts. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 231–248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0269993004200079 doi: 10.1080/02699930126213
  • Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Hillman, C. H., Hamm, A. O., & Lang, P. J. (2004). Brain processes in emotional perception: Motivated attention. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 593–611. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930341000239
  • Stevens, J. R., Volstorf, J., Schooler, L. J., & Rieskamp, J. (2010). Forgetting constrains the emergence of cooperative decision strategies. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, Article no. 235. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00235 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00203
  • Suzuki, A., Honma, Y., & Suga, S. (2013). Indelible distrust: Memory bias toward cheaters revealed as high persistence against extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1901–1913. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033335 23773183
  • Todorov, A., Gobbini, M. I., Evans, K. K., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Spontaneous retrieval of affective person knowledge in face perception. Neuropsychologia, 45, 163–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.018
  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57. doi: 10.1086/406755
  • Van Doorn, E. A., Heerdink, M. W., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Emotion and the construal of social situations: Inferences of cooperation versus competition from expressions of anger, happiness, and disappointment. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 442–461. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.648174 22471851 doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.648174
  • Van Lier, J., Revlin, R., & De Neys, W. (2013). Detecting cheaters without thinking: Testing the automaticity of the cheater detection module. PLoS ONE, 8, e53827. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053827
  • Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (2007). You can judge a book by its cover: The sequel. A kernel of truth in predictive cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 260–271.
  • Volstorf, J., Rieskamp, J., & Stevens, J. R. (2011). The good, the bad, and the rare: Memory for partners in social interactions. PLoS ONE, 6, e18945. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018945
  • Weimann, J. (1994). Individual behavior in a free riding experiment. Journal of Public Economics, 54, 185–200. doi: 10.1016/0047-2727(94)90059-0
  • Wentura, D., Müller, P., & Rothermund, K. (2014). Attentional capture by evaluative stimuli: Gain-and loss-connoting colors boost the additional-singleton effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 701–707. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0531-z 24488806 doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0531-z
  • Wentura, D., Rothermund, K., & Bak, P. (2000). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of approach-and avoidance-related social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1024–1037. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1024
  • Winkler, I., Jonas, K., & Rudolph, U. (2008). On the usefulness of memory skills in social interactions: Modifying the iterated prisoner's dilemma. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 52, 375–384. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022002707312606
  • Zaki, J., & Mitchell, J. P. (2013). Intuitive prosociality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 466–470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721413492764

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.