251
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

Emotional arousal does not modulate stimulus-response binding and retrieval effects

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 1509-1521 | Received 05 Jul 2022, Accepted 26 Sep 2022, Published online: 01 Oct 2022

References

  • Abrahamse, E., Braem, S., Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (2016). Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. Psychological Bulletin, 142(7), 693–728. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000047
  • Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 258–281. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.258
  • Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28(1), 403–450. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709
  • Bisby, J. A., Horner, A. J., Bush, D., & Burgess, N. (2018). Negative emotional content disrupts the coherence of episodic memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(2), 243–256. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000356
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9
  • Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tables. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 1–38. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.72
  • Colzato, L. S., Erasmus, V., & Hommel, B. (2004). Moderate alcohol consumption in humans impairs feature binding in visual perception but not across perception and action. Neuroscience Letters, 360(1-2), 103–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.01.054
  • Colzato, L. S., Fagioli, S., Erasmus, V., & Hommel, B. (2005). Caffeine, but not nicotine, enhances visual feature binding. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(2), 591–595. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03868.x
  • Dignath, D., & Eder, A. B. (2015). Stimulus conflict triggers behavioral avoidance. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(4), 822–836. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0355-6
  • Dignath, D., Eder, A. B., Steinhauser, M., & Kiesel, A. (2020). Conflict monitoring and the affective-signaling hypothesis—an integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(2), 193–216. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01668-9
  • Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012). Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain and Cognition, 78(2), 94–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.12.003
  • Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012). The role of affect and reward in the conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00342
  • Eder, A. B., Erle, T. M., & Kunde, W. (2020). Reward strengthens action–effect binding. Motivation Science, 6(3), 297–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000153
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A-G, & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
  • Frings, C., Hommel, B., Koch, I., Rothermund, K., Dignath, D., Giesen, C., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., Mayr, S., Moeller, B., Möller, M., Pfister, R., & Philipp, A. (2020). Binding and retrieval in action control (BRAC). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(5), 375–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.004
  • Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). To be or not to be … included in an event file: Integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus–response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5), 1209–1227. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023915
  • Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2017). How perception guides action: Figure-ground segmentation modulates integration of context features into S-R episodes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000403
  • Frings, C., Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2007). Distractor repetitions retrieve previous responses to targets. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(10), 1367–1377. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210600955645
  • Giesen, C., Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2012). Differences in the strength of distractor inhibition do not affect distractor-response bindings. Memory & Cognition, 40(3), 373–387. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0157-1
  • Giesen, C. G., Schmidt, J. R., & Rothermund, K. (2020). The law of recency: An episodic stimulus-response retrieval account of habit acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(Article 2927), 2927. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02927
  • Giesen, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). Affective matching moderates S-R binding. Cognition & Emotion, 25(2), 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.482765
  • Giesen, C., & Rothermund, K. (2015). Adapting to stimulus–response contingencies without noticing them. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(6), 1475–1481. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000122
  • Giesen, C., & Rothermund, K. (2016). Multi-level response coding in stimulus-response bindings: Irrelevant distractors retrieve both semantic and motor response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(10), 1643–1656. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000264
  • Hadley, C. B., & MacKay, D. G. (2006). Does emotion help or hinder immediate memory? Arousal versus priority-binding mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(1), 79–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.79
  • Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus–response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 376–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.004
  • Herbert, C., Kissler, J., Junghöfer, M., Peyk, P., & Rockstroh, B. (2006). Processing of emotional adjectives: Evidence from startle EMG and ERPs. Psychophysiology, 43(2), 197–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00385.x
  • Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1), 183–216. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756773
  • Hommel, B. (2007). Feature integration across perception and action: Event files affect response choice. Psychological Research, 71(1), 42–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0035-1
  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–878. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000103
  • Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K-PL. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y
  • Inzlicht, M., Bartholow, B. D., & Hirsh, J. B. (2015). Emotional foundations of cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 126–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.004
  • Janczyk, M., Giesen, C. G., Moeller, B., Dignath, D., & Pfister, R. (2022). Perception and action as viewed from the theory of event coding: A multi-lab replication and effect size estimation of common experimental designs. Psychological Research, Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01705-8
  • Kensinger, E. A. (2007). Negative emotion enhances memory accuracy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 213–218. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x
  • Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99–113. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908100432
  • Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Peyk, P., & Junghofer, M. (2007). Buzzwords. Psychological Science, 18(6), 475–480. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01924.x
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report, A-6.
  • Lee, T-H, Sakaki, M., Cheng, R., Velasco, R., & Mather, M. (2014). Emotional arousal amplifies the effects of biased competition in the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(12), 2067–2077. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu015
  • Lewis, P. A., Critchley, H. D., Rotshtein, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affective words. Cerebral Cortex, 17(3), 742–748. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhk024
  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
  • MacKay, D. G., Shafto, M., Taylor, J. K., Marian, D. E., Abrams, L., & Dyer, J. R. (2004). Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: Evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks. Memory & Cognition, 32(3), 474–488. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195840
  • Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(1), 33–52. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00028.x
  • Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., & Harley, C. W. (2016). Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E200. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000667
  • Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114–133. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611400234
  • Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6(5), 450–452. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1051
  • McGinnies, E. (1949). Emotionality and perceptual defense. Psychological Review, 56(5), 244–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056508
  • Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77(3), 249–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0435-y
  • Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2011). Remember the touch: Tactile distractors retrieve previous responses to targets. Experimental Brain Research, 214(1), 121–130. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2814-9
  • Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2014). Attention meets binding: Only attended distractors are used for the retrieval of event files. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(4), 959–978. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0648-9
  • Moeller, B., & Frings, C. (2017). Dissociation of binding and learning processes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(8), 2590–2605. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1393-7
  • Moeller, B., Frings, C., & Pfister, R. (2016). The structure of distractor-response bindings: Conditions for configural and elemental integration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(4), 464–479. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000158
  • Morsella, E., Wilson, L. E., Berger, C. C., Honhongva, M., Gazzaley, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). Subjective aspects of cognitive control at different stages of processing. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71(8), 1807–1824. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.8.1807
  • Payne, J. D., Nadel, L., Britton, W. B., & Jacobs, W. J. (2004). The biopsychology of trauma and memory. In D. Reisberg, & P. Hertel (Eds.), Series in affective science. Memory and emotion (pp. 76–128). Oxford University Press.
  • Questienne, L., van Dijck, J-P, & Gevers, W. (2018a). Introspection of subjective feelings is sensitive and specific. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(2), 215–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000437
  • Questienne, L., van Opstal, F., van Dijck, J-P, & Gevers, W. (2018b). Metacognition and cognitive control: Behavioural adaptation requires conflict experience. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(2), 411–423. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1251473
  • Schmalbrock, P., Laub, R., & Frings, C. (2021). Integrating salience and action – Increased integration strength through salience. Visual Cognition, 29(2), 91–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2020.1871455
  • Schmidt, J. R., Giesen, C. G., & Rothermund, K. (2020). Contingency learning as binding? Testing an exemplar view of the colour-word contingency learning effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(5), 739–761. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820906397
  • Sheth, B. R., & Pham, T. (2008). How emotional arousal and valence influence access to awareness. Vision Research, 48(23-24), 2415–2424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.013
  • Silvert, L., Delplanque, S., Bouwalerh, H., Verpoort, C., & Sequeira, H. (2004). Autonomic responding to aversive words without conscious valence discrimination. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 53(2), 135–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.03.005
  • Stürmer, B., Nigbur, R., Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2011). Reward and punishment effects on error processing and conflict control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2017-40069-001&site=ehost-live. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00335
  • Treisman, A. (1996). The binding problem. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6(2), 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80070-5
  • Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Addison-Wesley.
  • van der Ploeg, M. M., Brosschot, J. F., Versluis, A., & Verkuil, B. (2017). Peripheral physiological responses to subliminally presented negative affective stimuli: A systematic review. Biological Psychology, 129, 131–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.051
  • van Doorn, J., van den Bergh, D., Böhm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T., Etz, A., Evans, N. J., Gronau, Q. F., Haaf, J. M., Hinne, M., Kucharský, Š, Ly, A., Marsman, M., Matzke, D., Gupta, A. R. K. N., Sarafoglou, A., Stefan, A., Voelkel, J. G., & Wagenmakers, E-J. (2021). The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(3), 813–826. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01798-5
  • Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2008). Hebbian learning of cognitive control: Dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. Psychological Review, 115(2), 518–525. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.518
  • Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2009). Adaptation by binding: A learning account of cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(6), 252–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.02.007
  • Võ, ML-H, Conrad, M., Kuchinke, L., Urton, K., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). The Berlin affective word list reloaded (BAWL–R). Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 534–538. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.2.534
  • Weis, P. P., & Herbert, C. (2017). Bodily reactions to emotional words referring to own versus other people’s emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01277

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.