283
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Today’s positive affect predicts tomorrow’s experience of meaningful coincidences: a cross-lagged multilevel analysis

ORCID Icon, , &
Received 20 Oct 2023, Accepted 24 Apr 2024, Published online: 09 May 2024

References

  • Acar, S., & Sen, S. (2013). A multilevel meta-analysis of the relationship between creativity and schizotypy. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 214–228. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031975
  • Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106(3), 529–550. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.529. http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&NEWS=N&PAGE=fulltext&AN=00006832-199907000-00004&D=ovftd.
  • Baas, M., de Dreu, C., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 779–806. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012815
  • Beitman, B. D. (2009). Brains seek patterns in coincidences. Psychiatric Annals, 39(5), 255–264.
  • Beitman, B. D. (2011). Coincidence studies. Psychiatric Annals, 41(12), 561–571. https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20111104-03
  • Blain, S. D., Longenecker, J. M., Grazioplene, R. G., Klimes-Dougan, B., & DeYoung, C. G. (2020). Apophenia as the disposition to false positives: A unifying framework for openness and psychoticism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(3), 279–292. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000504
  • Boot, N., Baas, M., van Gaal, S., Cools, R., & de Dreu, C. (2017). Creative cognition and dopaminergic modulation of fronto-striatal networks: Integrative review and research agenda. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 78, 13–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.007
  • Bressan, P. (2002). The connection between random sequences, everyday coincidences, and belief in the paranormal. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16(1), 17–34. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.754
  • Brugger, P. (2001). From haunted brain to haunted science: A cognitive neuroscience view of paranormal and pseudoscientific thought. In J. Houran, & R. Lange (Eds.), Hauntings and poltergeists: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 195–213). McFarland & Company Inc. Publishers.
  • Brugger, P., & Graves, R. E. (1997). Testing vs. believing hypotheses: Magical ideation in the judgement of contingencies. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2(4), 251–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/135468097396270
  • Brugger, P., Landis, T., & Regard, M. (1990). A ‘sheep-goat effect’ in repetition avoidance: Extrasensory perception as an effect of subjective probability? British Journal of Psychology, 81(4), 455–468. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02372.x
  • Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80, 1–28. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01
  • Coleman, S. L., & Beitman, B. D. (2009). Characterizing high-frequency coincidence detectors. Psychiatric Annals, 39, 271–279. https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20090423-01
  • Conner, T. S., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Creative days: A daily diary study of emotion, personality, and everyday creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(4), 463–470. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000022
  • Cristofaro, M. (2021). Unfolding irrationality: How do meaningful coincidences influence management decisions? International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 29(2), 301–321. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJOA-01-2020-2010
  • de Dreu, C., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 739–756. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739
  • Diaconis, P., & Mosteller, F. (1989). Methods for studying coincidences. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 84(408), 853–861. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1989.10478847
  • Diana, L., Frei, M., Chesham, A., de Jong, D., Chiffi, K., Nyffeler, T., Bassetti, C. L., Goebel, N., Eberhard-Moscicka, A. K., & Müri, R. M. (2021). A divergent approach to pareidolias—Exploring creativity in a novel way. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 15(2), 313–323. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000293
  • Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 343–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.343
  • Folkman, S. (1984). Personal control and stress and coping processes: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 839–852. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.839
  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300
  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1367–1378. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1512
  • Fredrickson, B. L., Arizmendi, C., & van Cappellen, P. (2021). Same-day, cross-day, and upward spiral relations between positive affect and positive health behaviours. Psychology & Health, 36(4), 444–460. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2020.1778696
  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313–332. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000238
  • Gianotti, L. R., Mohr, C., Pizzagalli, D., Lehmann, D., & Brugger, P. (2001). Associative processing and paranormal belief. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 55(6), 595–603. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00911.x
  • Glatz, C., & Schwerdtfeger, A. (2022). Disentangling the causal structure between social trust, institutional trust, and subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 163(3), 1323–1348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-022-02914-9
  • Greyson, B. (2011). Meaningful coincidences and near-death experiences. Psychiatric Annals, 41, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20111104-09
  • Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102–116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889
  • Ivancovsky, T., Baror, S., & Bar, M. (2023). A shared novelty-seeking basis for creativity and curiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–61. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X23002807
  • Jauk, E. (2019). A bio-psycho-behavioral model of creativity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.012
  • Kammerer, P. (1919). Das Gesetz der Serie: Eine Lehre von den Wiederholungen im Lebens- und Weltgeschehen. Mit 8 Tafeln und 26 Abb. Deutsche Verlagsanstalt Stuttgart-Berlin.
  • Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., Szumski, G., & Firkowska-Mankiewicz, A. (2017). From moment-to-moment to day-to-day: Experience sampling and diary investigations in adults’ everyday creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(3), 309–324. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000127
  • Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C., & Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Diagnostica, 42, 139–156.
  • Lucas, R. E. (2023). Why the cross-lagged panel model is almost never the right choice. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 6, Article no. 25152459231158378, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459231158378
  • Matthews, R., & Blackmore, S. (1995). Why are coincidences so impressive? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80(Suppl. 3), 1121–1122. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1995.80.3c.1121
  • Mohr, C., Graves, R. E., Gianotti, L. R. R., Pizzagalli, D., & Brugger, P. (2001). Loose but normal: A semantic association study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(5), 475–483. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010461429079
  • Morrison, P. D., & Murray, R. M. (2009). From real-world events to psychosis: The emerging neuropharmacology of delusions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(4), 668–674. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp049
  • Nachman, G. (2009). Clinical implications of synchronicity and related phenomena. Psychiatric Annals, 39(5), 297–308. https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20090424-02. http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&NEWS=N&PAGE=fulltext&D=psyc&AN=2009-08205-005.
  • Nijstad, B. A., de Dreu, C., Rietzschel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21(1), 34–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463281003765323
  • Rominger, C. (2023, in press). The experience of meaningful coincidences is associated with more real-life creativity. PLOS One, https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/D9WT6
  • Rominger, C., Fink, A., & Perchtold-Stefan, C. M. (2023a). Experiencing more meaningful coincidences is associated with more real-life creativity? Insights from three empirical studies. Submitted for Publication.
  • Rominger, C., Fink, A., Perchtold-Stefan, C. M., Schulter, G., Weiss, E. M., & Papousek, I. (2022). Creative, yet not unique? Paranormal belief, but not self-rated creative ideation behavior is associated with a higher propensity to perceive unique meanings in randomness. Heliyon, 8, e09269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2022.e09269
  • Rominger, C., Fink, A., Weiss, E. M., Schulter, G., Perchtold, C. M., & Papousek, I. (2019). The propensity to perceive meaningful coincidences is associated with increased posterior alpha power during retention of information in a modified Sternberg paradigm. Consciousness and Cognition, 76, 102832. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.102832
  • Rominger, C., Perchtold-Stefan, C. M., & Fink, A. (2023b). The experience of meaningful coincidences is associated with stronger alpha power increases during an eyes-closed resting condition: A Bayesian replication approach. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 35(10), 1681–1692. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02033
  • Rominger, C., Schulter, G., Fink, A., Weiss, E. M., & Papousek, I. (2018). Meaning in meaninglessness: The propensity to perceive meaningful patterns in coincident events and randomly arranged stimuli is linked to enhanced attention in early sensory processing. Psychiatry Research, 263, 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.043
  • Rominger, C., Weiss, E. M., Fink, A., Schulter, G., & Papousek, I. (2011). Allusive thinking (cognitive looseness) and the propensity to perceive “meaningful” coincidences. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(8), 1002–1006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.012
  • Rossmann, E., & Fink, A. (2010). Do creative people use shorter associative pathways? Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 891–895. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.025
  • Roxburgh, E. C., Ridgway, S., & Roe, C. A. (2016). Synchronicity in the therapeutic setting: A survey of practitioners. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 16(1), 44–53. https://doi.org/10.1002/capr.12057
  • Russo-Netzer, P., & Icekson, T. (2022). An underexplored pathway to life satisfaction: The development and validation of the synchronicity awareness and meaning-detecting scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1053296. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1053296
  • Schuurman, N. K., Ferrer, E., Boer-Sonnenschein, M., & Hamaker, E. L. (2016). How to compare cross-lagged associations in a multilevel autoregressive model. Psychological Methods, 21(2), 206–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000062
  • Unger, I., Wabnegger, A., & Schienle, A. (2021). The association between the propensity to experience meaningful coincidence and brain anatomy in healthy females: The moderating role of coping skills. Consciousness and Cognition, 91, 103132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103132
  • Usami, S. (2021). On the differences between general cross-lagged panel model and random-intercept cross-lagged panel model: Interpretation of cross-lagged parameters and model choice. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(3), 331–344. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1821690
  • van Elk, M., Friston, K., & Bekkering, H. (2016). The experience of coincidence: An integrated psychological and neurocognitive perspective. In E. van Wolde, & K. Landsman (Eds.), The challenge of chance: A multidisciplinary approach from science and the humanities (pp. 171–185). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-26300-7_9
  • Wekenborg, M. K., Schwerdtfeger, A., Rothe, N., Penz, M., Walther, A., Kirschbaum, C., Thayer, J. F., Wittling, R. A., & Hill, L. K. (2022). Determining the direction of prediction of the association between parasympathetic dysregulation and exhaustion symptoms. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 10648. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-14743-4