457
Views
22
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Phonemic Awareness Contributes to Text Reading Fluency: Evidence From Eye MovementsFootnote

, , , & | (Action Editor)
Pages 157-170 | Received 10 Sep 2012, Accepted 13 Mar 2013, Published online: 27 Dec 2019

References

  • AdamsM. J. (1990). Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • AlonzoJ.TindalG.UlmerK.GlasgowA. (2006). easyCBM online progress monitoring assessment system. Eugene, OR: Center for Educational Assessment Accountability.
  • AshbyJ.RaynerK. (2004). Representing syllable information during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(3), 391–426.
  • AshbyJ.SandersL. D.KingstonJ. (2009). Skilled readers begin processing sub-phonemic features by 80 ms during visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 84–94.
  • AshbyJ.YangJ.EvansK. H. C.RaynerK. (2012). Eye movements and the perceptual span in silent and oral reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(4), 634–640.
  • BeckI. L. (2006). Making sense of phonics: The hows and whys. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • BuswellG. T. (1922). Fundamental reading habits: A study of their development. (Supplemental Education Monograph.) Chicago, IL: University of Chicago.
  • CassarM.TreimanR. (1997). The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children's knowledge of double letters in words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), 631–644.
  • ChallJ. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • ChallJ. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • ChallJ. S.JacobsV. A.BaldwinL. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • CunninghamA. E.PerryK. E.StanovichK. E. (2001). Converging evidence for the concept of orthographic processing. Reading and Writing, 14(5–6), 549–568.
  • DehaeneS. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York, NY: Penguin Viking.
  • DencklaM. B.RudelR. G. (1976). Naming of objects by dyslexic and other learning-disabled children. Brain and Language, 3, 1–15.
  • de JongP. F.VrielinkL. O. (2004). Rapid automatic naming: Easy to measure, hard to improve (quickly). Annals of Dyslexia, 54(1), 65–88.
  • DowhowerS. L. (1987). Effects of repeated reading on second-grade transitional readers' fluency and comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 389–406.
  • EhriL. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In GoughP. B.EhriL. C.TreimanR. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • EhriL. (2002). Phases of acquisition in learning to read words and implications for teaching. In StainthorpR.TomlinsonP. (Eds.), Learning and teaching reading. (Monograph Series II.) London, UK: British Journal of Educational Psychology.
  • FeltonR. H.WoodF. B.BrownI. S.CampbellS. K.HarterM. R. (1987). Separate verbal memory and naming deficits in attention deficit disorder and reading disability. Brain and Language, 31(1), 171–184.
  • FuchsL. S.FuchsD.HospM. K.JenkinsJ. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239–256.
  • FuchsL. S.FuchsD.MaxwellL. (1988). The validity of informal measures of reading comprehension. Remedial & Special Education, 9(2), 20–28.
  • GoodR. H.SimmonsD. C.Kame′enuiE. (2001). The importance and decision-making utility of a continuum of fluency-based indicators of foundational reading skills for third-grade high-stakes outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 257–288.
  • GoodR. H.SimmonsD. C.SmithS. (1998). Effective academic intervention in the United States: Evaluating and enhancing the acquisition of early reading skills. School Psychology Review, 27, 45–56.
  • HaldermanL. K.AshbyJ.PerfettiC. A. (2012). Phonology: An early and integral role in identifying words. In AdelmanJ. (Ed.), Visual word recognition, Volume I: Models and methods, orthography and phonology (pp. 207–228). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • HyönäJ.OlsonR. K. (1995). Eye fixation patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: Effects of word length and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(6), 1430–1440.
  • JormA.ShareD. (1983). Phonological recoding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103–147.
  • JormA. F.ShareD. L.MacleanR.MatthewsR. G. (1984). Phonological recoding skills and learning to read: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5(3), 201–207.
  • KatzirT.KimY.WolfM.O'BrienB.KennedyB.LovettM.MorrisR. (2006). Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56(1), 51–82.
  • KirkbyJ. A.WebsterL. A. D.BlytheH. I.LiversedgeS. P. (2008). Binocular coordination during reading and non-reading tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 134(5), 742–763.
  • KorhonenT. (1995). The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with learning difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 232–239.
  • KuhnM. R.StahlS. A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 3–21.
  • LaBergeD.SamuelsS. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6(2), 293–323.
  • LervågA.HulmeC. (2009). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a mechanism that places constraints on the development of early reading fluency. Psychological Science, 20(8), 1040–1048.
  • LevyB. A.AbelloB.LysynchukL. (1997). Transfer from word training to reading in context: Gains in reading fluency and comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly, 20(3), 173–188.
  • LibermanI. Y. (1973). Segmentation of the spoken word and reading acquisition. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 23, 65–77.
  • LibermanI. Y.ShankweilerD.FischerF. W.CarterB. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18(2), 201–212.
  • MattinglyI. G. (1972). Reading, the linguistic process, and linguistic awareness. In KavanaghJ. F.MattinglyI. G. (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye: The relationship between speech and reading. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Melby-LervågM.LysterS. H.HulmeC. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322–352.
  • MeyerM. S.WoodF. B.HartL. A.FeltonR. H. (1998). Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 91–114.
  • NagyW.AndersonR. (1984). The number of words in printed school English. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.
  • NathanR. G.StanovichK. E. (1991). The causes and consequences of differences in reading fluency. Theory Into Practice, 30, 176–184.
  • NationK.CockseyJ. (2009). The relationship between knowing a word and reading it aloud in children's word reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(3), 296–308.
  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
  • PerfettiC. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • PerfettiC. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In EhriL. C.TreimanR.GoughP. B. (Ed.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • PerfettiC. A.HartL. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In VehoevenL.ElbroC.ReitsmaP. (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
  • RaynerK. (1978). Eye movements in reading and information processing. Psychological Bulletin, 85(3), 618–660.
  • RaynerK. (1986). Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41(2), 211–236.
  • RaynerK. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.
  • RaynerK. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506.
  • RaynerK.FoormanB. R.PerfettiC. A.PesetskyD.SeidenbergM. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2(2), 31–74.
  • RaynerK.PollatsekA.AshbyJ.CliftonC. E. (2012). The psychology of reading. London, UK: Psychology Press.
  • ShaywitzS. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia: A new and complete science-based program for reading problems at any level. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
  • ShareD. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218.
  • StanovichK. E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.
  • StanovichK. E. (1984). The interactive-compensatory model of reading: A confluence of developmental, experimental, and educational psychology. RASE: Remedial & Special Education, 5(3), 11–19.
  • StanovichK. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360–407.
  • StanovichK. E.SiegelL. S. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.
  • TinkerM. A. (1958). Recent studies of eye movements in reading. Psychological Bulletin, 55(4), 215–231.
  • TorgesenJ. K. (2000). Individual responses in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15, 55–64.
  • TorgesenJ. K.HudsonR. (2006). Reading fluency: critical issues for struggling readers. In SamuelsS. J.FarstrupA. (Eds.), Reading fluency: The forgotten dimension of reading success (pp. 130–158). Newark, DE: International Reading Association
  • TorgesenJ. K.RashotteC. A.AlexanderA. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In WolfM. (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 333–355). Parkton, MD: York Press.
  • VellutinoF. R.FletcherJ. M.SnowlingM. J.ScanlonD. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40.
  • WagnerR. K.TorgesenJ. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 192–212.
  • WolfM.Katzir-CohenT. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 211–239.
  • ZenoS.IvensS.MillardR.DuvvuriR. (1995). The educator's word frequency guide. Brewster, NY: Touchstone Applied Science Associates.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.