Publication Cover
Experimental Aging Research
An International Journal Devoted to the Scientific Study of the Aging Process
Volume 45, 2019 - Issue 1
499
Views
20
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The Effect of Aging on Memory for Recent and Remote Egocentric and Allocentric Information

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 57-73 | Received 28 Aug 2017, Accepted 07 Dec 2018, Published online: 27 Dec 2018

References

  • Agresti, A., & Min, Y. (2005). Frequentist performance of Bayesian confidence intervals for comparing proportions in 2 × 2 contingency tables. Biometrics, 61, 2. doi:10.1111/j.1541-0420.2005.031228.x
  • Aguirre, G. K., & D’Esposito, M. (1999). Topographical disorientation: A synthesis and taxonomy. Brain, 122, 1613–1628.
  • Allison, S. L., Fagan, A. M., Morris, J. C., & Head, D. (2016). Spatial Navigation in Preclinical Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 52(1), 77–90.
  • Antova, L., Koch, C., & Olteanu, D. (2009). 10(10)6 worlds and beyond: Efficient representation and processing of incomplete information. Very Large Data Bases Journal, 18, 5. doi:10.1007/s00778-009-0149-y
  • Barrash, J. (1994). Age-related decline in route learning ability. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 3. doi:10.1080/87565649409540578
  • Begega, A., Cienfuegos, S., Rubio, S., Santín, J. L., Miranda, R., & Arias, J. L. (2001). Effects of ageing on allocentric and egocentric spatial strategies in the Wistar rat. Behavioral Processes, 53, 1–2. doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(00)00150-9
  • Bosco, A., Longoni, A. M., & Vecchi, T. (2004). Gender effects in spatial orientation: Cognitive profiles and mental strategies. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(5), 519–532. doi:10.1002/acp.1000
  • Bosco, A., Picucci, L., Caffò, A. O., Lancioni, G. E., & Gyselinck, V. (2008). Assessing human reorientation ability inside virtual reality environments: The effects of retention interval and landmark characteristics. Cognitive Processing, 9(4), 299–309. doi:10.1007/s10339-008-0210-6
  • Bosco, A., Spano, G., Caffò, A. O., Lopez., A., Grattagliano., I., Saracino, G., … Lancioni, G. E. (2017). Italians do it worse. Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) optimal cut-off scores for people with probable Alzheimer’s disease and with probable cognitive impairment. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 1–8. doi:10.1007/s40520-017-0727
  • Brink, T. A., Yesavage, J. A., Lum, O., Heersema, P., Adey, M., & Rose, T. L. (1982). Screening tests for geriatric depression. Gerontologist, 1, 37–44. doi:10.1300/J018v01n01_06
  • Burgess, N., Becker, S., King, J. A., & O’Keefe, J. (2001). Memory for events and their spatial context: Models and experiments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 356(1413), 1493–1503. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.0948
  • Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., & O’Keefe, J. (2002). The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron, 35, 625–641.
  • Caffò, A. O., De Caro, M. F., Picucci, L., Notarnicola, A., Settanni, A., Livrea, P., … Bosco, A. (2012). Reorientation deficits are associated with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 27, 321–330. doi:10.1177/1533317512452035
  • Caffò, A. O., Hoogeveen, F., Groenendaal, M., Perilli, A. V., Picucci, L., Lancioni, G. E., & Bosco, A. (2014a). Intervention strategies for spatial orientation disorders in dementia: A selective review. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 17(3), 200–209. doi:10.3109/17518423.2012.749951
  • Caffò, A. O., Hoogeveen, F., Groenendaal, M., Perilli, V. A., Damen, M., Stasolla, F., & Bosco, A. (2014b). Comparing two different orientation strategies for promoting indoor traveling in people with Alzheimer’s disease. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(2), 572–580. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.12.003
  • Caffò, A. O., Lopez, A., Spano, G., Serino, S., Cipresso, P., Stasolla, F., … Bosco, A. (2017). Spatial reorientation declines in aging: The combination of geometry and landmarks. Aging & Mental Health. doi:10.1080/13607863.2017.1354973
  • Campbell, J. I., Hepner, I. J., & Miller, L. A. (2014). The influence of age and sex on memory for a familiar environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 40, 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.04.007
  • Carlesimo, G. A., Mauri, M., Graceffa, A. M., Fadda, L., Loasses, A., Lorusso, S., & Caltagirone, C. (1998). Memory performances in young, elderly, and very old healthy individuals versus patients with Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence for discontinuity between normal and pathological aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 14–29. doi:10.1076/jcen.20.1.14.1482
  • Ciaramelli, E., Rosenbaum, R. S., Solcz, S., Levine, B., & Moscovitch, M. (2010). Mental space travel: Damage to posterior parietal cortex prevents egocentric navigation and reexperiencing of remote spatial memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 619.
  • Colombo, D., Serino, S., Tuena, C., Pedroli, E., Dakanalis, A., Cipresso, P., & Riva, G. (2017). Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 605–621. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.012
  • Coluccia, E., Bosco, A., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2007). The role of visuo-spatial working memory in map learning: New findings from a map-drawing paradigm. Psychological Research, 71, 3. doi:10.1007/s00426-006-0090-2
  • Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 329–340. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.08.006
  • Evans, G., Brennan, P., Skorpanich, M., & Held, D. (1984). Cognitive mapping and elderly adults: Verbal and location memory for urban landmarks. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 452–457. doi:10.1093/geronj/39.4.452
  • Fenollar, P., Román, S., & Cuestas, P. J. (2007). University students’ academic performance: An integrative conceptual framework and empirical analysis. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(4), 873–891. doi:10.1348/000709907X189118
  • Foley, J. E., & Cohen, A. J. (1984). Working mental representations of the environment. Environment and Behavior, 16, 6. doi:10.1177/0013916584166003
  • Fox, J., Friendly, M., & Weisberg, S. (2013). Hypothesis tests for multivariate linear models using the car package. The R Journal, 5(1), 39–52.
  • Galea, L. A. M., & Kimura, D. (1993). Sex differences in route-learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 1. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90174-2
  • Gazova, I., Vlcek, K., Laczó, J., Nedelska, Z., Hyncicova, E., Mokrisova, I., … Hort, J. (2012). Spatial navigation—A unique window into physiological and pathological aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 4. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2012.00016
  • Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel hierarchical models (Vol. 1). New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Giudice, N. A., Bennett, C. R., Klatzky, R. L., & Loomis, J. M. (2017). Spatial updating of haptic arrays across the life span. Experimental Aging Research, 43(3), 274–290. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2017.1298958
  • Hartley, T., Lever, C., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2013). Space in the brain: How the hippocampal formation supports spatial cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 1635. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0510
  • Head, D., & Isom, M. (2010). Age effects on wayfinding and route learning skills. Behavioral Brain Research, 209, 1. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.012
  • Hirshhorn, M., Newman, L., & Moscovitch, M. (2011). Detailed descriptions of routes traveled, but not map-like knowledge, correlates with tests of hippocampal function in older adults. Hippocampus, 21, 11. doi:10.1002/hipo.20871
  • Iachini, T., Ruggiero, G., & Ruotolo, F. (2009a). The effect of age on egocentric and allocentric spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Processing, 10(SUPPL), 2. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0276-9
  • Iachini, T., Ruotolo, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2009b). The effects of familiarity and gender on spatial representation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(2), 227–234. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.07.001
  • Iaria, G., Petrides, M., Dagher, A., Pike, B., & Bohbot, V. D. (2003). Cognitive strategies dependent on the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in human navigation: Variability and change with practice. The Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 23(13), 5945–5952.
  • Katz, S. (1983). Assessing self-maintenance: Activities of daily living, mobility, and instrumental activities of daily living. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 31(12), 721–727.
  • Kirasic, K. C. (1991). Spatial cognition and behavior in young and elderly adults: Implications for learning new environments. Psychology and Aging, 6, 1. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.6.1.10
  • Kirasic, K. C., Allen, G. L., & Haggerty, D. (1992). Age-related differences in adults’ macrospatial cognitive processes. Experimental Aging Research, 18, 33–39. doi:10.1080/03610739208253914
  • Kirasic, K. C., Allen, G. L., & Siegel, A. W. (1984). Expression of configurational knowledge of Large-Scale environments: Students’ Performance of Cognitive Tasks. Environment and Behavior, 16(6), 687–712. doi:10.1177/0013916584166002
  • Klatzsky, R. L. (1998). Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Representations: Definitions, Distinctions, and Interconnections .  In Spatial cognition (pp. 1–17). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). On cognitive neuroscience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 3. doi:10.1162/jocn.1994.6.3.297
  • Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist, 9(3), 179–181.
  • Lithfous, S., Dufour, A., Blanc, F., & Després, O. (2014). Allocentric but not egocentric orientation is impaired during normal aging: an ERP study. Neuropsychology, 28, 761–771. doi:10.1037/neu0000084
  • Maguire, E. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Mishkin, M. (2001). The effects of bilateral hippocampal damage on fMRI regional activations and interactions during memory retrieval. Brain, 124, 1156–1170. doi:10.1093/brain/124.6.1156
  • Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K., & Spiers, H. J. (2006). London taxi drivers and bus drivers: A structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus, 16, 12. doi:10.1002/hipo.20233
  • Mangels, J. A., Butterfield, B., Lamb, J., Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Why do beliefs about intelligence influence learning success? A social cognitive neuroscience model. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(2), 75–86. doi:10.1093/scan/nsl013
  • Marr, D., & Nishihara, H. K. (1978). Representation and recognition of three-dimensional shapes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, 200(1140), 269–294. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1978.0020
  • McNamara, T. P., Rump, B., & Werner, S. (2003). Egocentric and geocentric frames of reference in memory of large-scale space. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 3. doi:10.3758/BF03196519
  • Merriman, N. A., Ondřej, J., Rybicki, A., Roudaia, E., O’Sullivan, C., & Newell, F. N. (2016). Crowded environments reduce spatial memory in older but not younger adults. Psychological Research, 1–22. doi:10.1007/s00426-016-0819-5
  • Meulenbroek, O., Petersson, K. M., Voermans, N., Weber, B., & Fernández, G. (2004). Age differences in neural correlates of route encoding and route recognition. Neuroimage, 22(4), 1503–1514. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.007
  • Moffat, S. D., Elkins, W., & Resnick, S. M. (2006). Age differences in the neural systems supporting human allocentric spatial navigation. Neurobiology of Aging, 27(7), 965–972. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.011
  • Moffat, S. D., & Resnick, S. M. (2002). Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 5. doi:10.1037//0735-7044.116.5.851
  • Moffat, S. D., Zonderman, A. B., & Resnick, S. M. (2001). Age differences in spatial memory in a virtual environment navigation task. Neurobiology of Aging, 22, 5. doi:10.1016/S0197-4580(01)00251-2
  • Montello, D. R., Lovelace, K. L., Golledge, R. G., & Self, C. M. (1999). Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 89, 3. doi:10.1111/0004-5608.00160
  • Moscovitch, M., Nadel, L., Winocur, G., Gilboa, A., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2006). The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 179–190. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.013
  • Moscovitch, M., Rosenbaum, R. S., Addis, D. R., Westmacott, R., Grady, C., McAndrews, M. P., … Nadel, L. (2005a). Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: A unified account based on multiple trace theory. Journal of Anatomy, 207(1), 35–66. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x
  • Moscovitch, M., Westmacott, R., Gilboa, A., Addis, D. R., Rosenbaum, R. S., Viskontas, I., … Gao, F. (2005b). Hippocampal complex contribution to retention and retrieval of recent and remote episodic and semantic memories: Evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging studies of healthy and brain-damaged people. In Dynamic cognitive processes (pp. 333–380). Tokyo: Springer.
  • Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., & Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695–699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x
  • O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Pastore, M. (2015). Analisi dei dati in psicologia. Bologna: Il Mulino.
  • Picucci, L., Bosco, A., Caffò, A. O., D’Angelo, G., Soleti, E., Lancioni, G. E., Di Masi, M. N. (2010). Picucci, L., Bosco, A., Caffò A.O., D’Angelo, G., Soleti, E., Lancioni, G.E., Di Masi, M.N. (2010). A New Methodology to Assess Individual Differences in Spatial Memory: The Computer-Generated Version of the Reorientation Paradigm. In Salvati, G., Rabuano, V. (Eds.), Cognitive Psychology Perspectives. Nova Science Publishers, Hauppage NY, pp. 159-196.
  • Picucci, L., Caffò, A. O., & Bosco, A. (2009). Age and sex differences in a virtual version of the reorientation task. Cognitive Processing, 10(2), 272–275. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0321-8
  • Picucci, L., Caffò, A. O., & Bosco, A. (2011). Besides navigation accuracy: Gender differences in strategy selection and level of spatial confidence. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31(4), 430–438. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.01.005
  • Postma, A., Jager, G., Kessels, R. P. C., Koppeschaar, H. P. F., & van Honk, J. (2004). Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain Cognition, 54, 24–34. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00238-0
  • Prestopnik, J. L., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, B. (2000). The relations among wayfinding strategy use, sense of direction, sex, familiarity, and wayfinding ability. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20(2). doi:10.1006/jevp.1999.016
  • Richardson, K., & Norgate, S. H. (2015). Does IQ really predict job performance? Applied Developmental Science, 19(3), 153–169. doi:10.1080/10888691.2014.983635
  • Rosenbaum, R. S., Gao, F., Richards, B., Black, S. E., & Moscovitch, M. (2005). ‘Where to?’ remote memory for spatial relations and landmark identity in former taxi drivers with Alzheimer’s disease and encephalitis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 446–462. doi:10.1162/0898929053279496
  • Rosenbaum, R. S., Priselac, S., Köhler, S., Black, S. E., Gao, F., Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. (2000). Remote spatial memory in an amnesic person with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions. Nature Neuroscience, 3(10), 1044–1048. doi:10.1038/79867
  • Rosenbaum, R. S., Winocur, G., Binns, M. A., & Moscovitch, M. (2012). Remote spatial memory in aging: All is not lost. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 4. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2012.00025
  • Rosseel, Y. (2011). Lavan: A R package for structural equation modelling and more. http://users.ugent.be/~yrosseel/lavaan/lavaanIntroduction.pdf ( accessed on 11 July 2011).
  • Ruggiero, G., D’Errico, O., & Iachini, T. (2015). Development of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations from childhood to elderly age. Psychological Research, 80, 2. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0658-9
  • Ruggiero, G., Iachini, T., & Ruotolo, G. (2009). Cognitive Psychology Perspective. In G. Salvati & V. Rabuano (Eds.), Cognitive Psychology Perspective. Spatial memory: The role egocentric and allocentric spatial frames of reference (pp. 1–25). Hauppage,New York: Nova Publishers.
  • Ruggiero, G., Sergi, I., & Iachini, T. (2008). Gender differences in remembering and inferring spatial distances. Memory, 16, 8. doi:10.1080/09658210802307695
  • Serino, S., Cipresso, P., Morganti, F., & Riva, G. (2014). The role of egocentric and allocentric abilities in Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review. Ageing Research Reviews, 16(1), 32–44. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2014.04.004
  • Serino, S., Morganti, F., Di Stefano, F., & Riva, G. (2015). Detecting early egocentric and allocentric impairments deficits in Alzheimer’s disease: An experimental study with virtual reality. Frontiers Aging Neuroscience, 7, 88. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2015.00088
  • Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology, 14(4), 560–589.
  • Tlauka, M. (2006). Orientation dependent mental representations following real-world navigation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47(3), 171–176. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00504.x
  • Van der Borght, K., Havekes, R., Bos, T., Eggen, B. J., & Van der Zee, E. A. (2007). Exercise improves memory acquisition and retrieval in the Y-maze task: Relationship with hippocampal neurogenesis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121(2), 324. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.324
  • Waller, D. (2005). The WALKABOUT: Using virtual environments to assess large-scale spatial abilities. Computers in Human Behavior, 21(2), 243–253. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2004.02.022
  • Waller, D., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., & Hegarty, M. (2002). Orientation Specificity and Spatial Updating of Memories for Layouts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 28(6), 1051.
  • WHOQOL Group. (1995). The World Health Organization Quality of Life assessment (WHOQOL): Position paper from the World Health Organization. Social Science & Medicine, 41(10), 1403–1409. doi:10.1016/0277-9536(95)00112-K
  • Winocur, G., Moscovitch, M., & Sekeres, M. (2007). Memory consolidation or transformation: Context manipulation and hippocampal representations of memory. Nature Neuroscience, 10(5), 555–557. doi:10.1038/nn1880
  • Youn, J. C., Kim, K. W., Lee, D. Y., Jhoo, J. H., Lee, S. B., Park, J. H., … Woo, J. I. (2009). Development of the subjective memory complaints questionnaire. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 27, 310–317. doi:10.1159/000205512

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.