999
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Neutralization, Moral Disengagement, and Delinquency in Adolescence: Testing the Reciprocal Effects of Proactive Criminal Thinking and Guilt on Future Offending

ORCID Icon
Pages 210-230 | Received 14 May 2018, Accepted 13 Oct 2018, Published online: 25 Jan 2019

References

  • Aleixo, P. A., & Norris, C. E. (2000). Personality and moral reasoning in young offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(3), 609–623.
  • Allison, P. D. (2012). Handling missing data by maximum likelihood. SAS Global Forum 2012 (Paper 312-2012). Cary, NC: SAS Institute.
  • Antonaccio, O., & Tittle, C. R. (2008). Morality, self-control, and crime. Criminology, 46(2), 479–510.
  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Bandura, A. (1990). Mechanisms of moral disengagement. In W. Reich (Ed.), Origins of terrorism: Psychologies, ideologies, theologies, and states of mind (pp. 161–191). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Theory, research and applications (Vol.1, pp. 71–129). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 364–374.
  • Bao, Z., Zhang, W., Lai, X., Sun, W., & Wang, Y. (2015). Parental attachment and Chinese adolescents’ delinquency: The mediating role of moral disengagement. Journal of Adolescence, 44, 37–47.
  • Capage, L., & Watson, A. C. (2001). Individual differences in theory of mind, aggressive behavior, and social skills in young children. Early Education and Development, 12(4), 613–628.
  • Cechaviciute, I., & Kenny, D. T. (2007). The relationship between neutralizations and perceived delinquent labeling on criminal history in young offenders serving community orders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34(6), 816–829.
  • Chen, C.-A., & Howitt, D. (2007). Different crime types and moral reasoning development in young offenders compared with non-offenders. Psychology, Crime and Law, 13(4), 405–416.
  • Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 558–577.
  • Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., & Kam, C. M. (2001). A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6(4), 330–351.
  • Elwert, F., & Winship, C. (2014). Endogenous selection bias: The problem of conditioning on a collider variable. Annual Review of Sociology, 40, 31–53.
  • Esbensen, F.-A. (2002, September). National evaluation of the Gang Resistance Education and Training (GREAT) program: Final Report (94-IJ-CX-0058). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
  • Fontaine, R. G., Fida, R., Paciello, M., Tisak, M. S., & Caprara, G. V. (2014). The mediating role of moral disengagement in the developmental course from peer rejection in adolescence to crime in early adulthood. Psychology, Crime and Law, 20(1), 1–19.
  • Fritsche, I. (2005). Predicting deviant behavior by neutralization: Myths and findings. Deviant Behavior, 26(5), 483–510.
  • Gault-Sherman, M. (2012). It’s a two-way street: The bidirectional relationship between parenting and delinquency. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(2), 121–145.
  • Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science (New York, N.Y.), 316(5827), 998–1002.
  • Haidt, J., Graham, J., & Joseph, C. (2009). Above and below left–right: Ideological narratives and moral foundations. Psychological Inquiry, 20(2–3), 110–119.
  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 55–66.
  • Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., & Moilanen, K. L. (2010). Developmental precursors of moral disengagement and the role of moral disengagement in the development of antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(2), 197–209.
  • Imai, K., Keele, L., & Tingley, D. (2010). A general approach to causal mediation analysis. Psychological Methods, 15(4), 309–334.
  • Jacobs, B. A., & Copes, H. (2015). Neutralization without drift: Criminal commitment among persistent offenders. British Journal of Criminology, 55(2), 286–302.
  • Kenny, D. A. (2013). Mediation: Sensitivity analysis [webinar]. Retrieved from http://davidakenny.net/webinars/Mediation/Sensitivity/Sensitivity.html
  • Kohlberg, L. (1981). Essays on moral development, I: The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row.
  • Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development, II: The psychology of moral development. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row.
  • Landenberger, N. A., & Lipsey, M. W. (2005). The positive effects of cognitive-behavioral programs for offenders: A meta-analysis of factors associated with effective treatment. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 1(4), 451–476.
  • Martinez, A. G., Stuewig, J., & Tangney, J. P. (2014). Can perspective-taking reduce crime? Examining a pathway through empathic-concern and guilt-proneness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(12), 1659–1667.
  • Maruna, S., & Copes, H. (2005). What have we learned from five decades of neutralization research? Crime and Justice, 32, 221–320.
  • Matza, D. (1964). Delinquency and drift. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Maxwell, S. R., Zepeda, A., & Rzotkiewicz, A. (2018). Peers, social control, or guilt? Self-conscious emotions as explanation for antisocial behavior. Deviant Behavior, 39(3), 322–331.
  • Meldrum, R. C., Young, J. T. N., Hay, C., & Flexon, J. L. (2012). Does self-control influence maternal attachment? A reciprocal effects analysis from early childhood through middle adolescence. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 28(4), 673–699.
  • Mori, A., & Cigala, A. (2016). Perspective taking: Training procedures in developmentally typical preschoolers. Different intervention methods and their effectiveness. Educational Psychology Review, 28(2), 267–294.
  • Morris, R. G., & Copes, H. (2012). Exploring the temporal dynamics of the neutralization/delinquency relationship. Criminal Justice Review, 37(4), 442–460.
  • Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén.
  • Palmer, E. J., & Hollin, C. R. (1998). A comparison of patterns of moral development in young offenders and non-offenders. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3(2), 225–235.
  • Peyre, H., Leplége, A., & Coste, J. (2011). Missing data methods for dealing with missing items in quality of life questionnaires: A comparison by simulation of personal mean score, full information maximum likelihood, multiple imputation, and hot deck techniques applied to the SF-36 in the French 2003 decennial health survey. Quality of Life Research, 20(2), 287–300.
  • Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York, NY: Free Press.
  • Piquero, A. R., Jennings, W. G., Diamond, B., Farrington, D. P., Tremblay, R. E., Welsh, B. C., & Gonzalez, J. M. R. (2016). A meta-analysis update on the effects of early family/parent training programs on antisocial behavior and delinquency. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 12(2), 229–248.
  • Preacher, K. J. (2015). Advances in mediation analysis: A survey and synthesis of new developments. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 825–852.
  • Preacher, K. J., & Selig, J. P. (2012). Advantages of Monte Carlo confidence intervals for indirect effects. Communication Methods and Measures, 6(2), 77–98.
  • Ribeaud, D., & Eisner, M. (2010). Are moral disengagement, neutralization techniques, and self-serving cognitive distortions the same? Developing a unified scale of moral neutralization of aggression. International Journal of Conflict and Violence, 4, 298–315.
  • Roos, S., Hodges, E. V. E., & Salmivalli, C. (2014). Do guilt- and shame-proneness differentially predict prosocial, aggressive, and withdrawn behaviors during early adolescence? Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 941–946.
  • Shulman, E. P., Cauffman, E., Piquero, A. R., & Fagan, J. (2011). Moral disengagement among serious juvenile offenders: A longitudinal study of the relations between morally disengaged attitudes and offending. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1619–1632.
  • Silver, E., & Abell, L. (2016). Beyond harm and fairness: A study of deviance and morality. Deviant Behavior, 37(5), 496–508.
  • Spruit, A., Schalkwijk, F., van Vugt, E., & Stams, G. J. (2016). The relation between self-conscious emotions and delinquency: A meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 28, 12–20.
  • Sutherland, E. H. (1947). Principles of criminology (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: J.B. Lippincott.
  • Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22(6), 664–670.
  • Tangney, J. P. (1999). The self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), A handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 541–568). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345–372.
  • Thornberry, T. P. (1987). Toward an interactional theory of delinquency. Criminology, 25, 863–891.
  • Thornberry, T. P., Lizotte, A. J., Krohn, M. D., Farnworth, M., & Jang, S. J. (1994). Delinquent peers, beliefs, and delinquent behavior: A longitudinal test of interactional theory. Criminology, 32(1), 47–83.
  • Topalli, V. (2005). When being good is bad: An expansion of neutralization theory. Criminology, 43(3), 797–836.
  • Vasquez, A., & Vieraitis, L. M. (2016). “It’s just paint”: Street taggers’ use of neutralization techniques. Deviant Behavior, 37(10), 1179–1195.
  • Walters, G. D. (2015). Proactive criminal thinking and the transmission of differential association: A cross-lagged multi-wave path analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 42(11), 1128–1144.
  • Walters, G. D. (2016a). Criminal thought content and criminal thought process as mediators of peer influence: Combined versus individual effects. Criminal Justice Review, 41(3), 318–334.
  • Walters, G. D. (2016b). Friends, cognition, and delinquency: Proactive and reactive criminal thinking as mediators of the peer influence and peer selection effects. Justice Quarterly, 33(6), 1055–1079.
  • Walters, G. D. (2017a). Modelling the criminal lifestyle: Theorizing at the edge of chaos. London, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Walters, G. D. (2017b). Proactive criminal thinking and deviant identity as mediators of the peer influence effect. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 15(3), 281–298.
  • Walters, G. D. (2018). Personality and crime: Mediating the agreeableness–offending and conscientiousness–offending relationships with proactive and reactive criminal thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 129, 166–170.
  • Walters, G. D., & Yurvati, E. (2017). Testing the construct validity of the PICTS proactive and reactive scores against six putative measures of proactive and reactive criminal thinking. Psychology, Crime & Law, 23(1), 1–14.
  • Wiesner, M. (2003). A longitudinal latent variable analysis of reciprocal relations between depressive symptoms and delinquency during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 633–645.
  • Wikström, P. H. (2006). Individuals, settings, and acts of crime: Situational mechanisms and the explanation of crime. In P. H. Wikström & R. J. Sampson (Eds.), The explanation of crime (pp. 61–107). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Wilson, D. B., Bouffard, L. A., & MacKenzie, D. L. (2005). A quantitative review of structured, group-oriented, cognitive–behavioral programs for offenders. Journal of Criminal Justice and Behavior, 32(2), 172–204.
  • Wu, A. D., & Zumbo, B. D. (2008). Understanding and using mediators and moderators. Social Indicators Research, 87(3), 367–392.
  • Yoo, J. A. (2017). Developmental changes in the bidirectional relationships between parental monitoring and child delinquency. Children and Youth Services Review, 73, 360–367.
  • Young, R., & Johnson, D. R. (2013). Methods for handling missing secondary respondent data. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 75(1), 221–234.
  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151–175.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.