Publication Cover
Chronobiology International
The Journal of Biological and Medical Rhythm Research
Volume 37, 2020 - Issue 4
1,711
Views
14
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Circadian rhythms and decision-making: a review and new evidence from electroencephalography

ORCID Icon, , , &
Pages 520-541 | Received 29 Nov 2019, Accepted 06 Jan 2020, Published online: 31 Jan 2020

References

  • Adan A, Almirall H. 1991. Horne and Östberg Morningness–eveningness questionnaire: a reduced scale. Pers Individ Differ. 12:241–253. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W.
  • Adan A, Archer SN, Hidalgo MP, Di Milia L, Natale V, Randler C. 2012. Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiol Int. 29(9):1153–1175. doi:10.3109/07420528.2012.719971.
  • Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. 1990. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci. 52(1–2):29–37. doi:10.3109/00207459008994241.
  • Anderson C, Dickinson DL. 2010. Bargaining and trust: the effects of 36-h total sleep deprivation on socially interactive decisions. J Sleep Res. 19(1 Pt 1):54–63. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00767.x.
  • Berdynaj D, Boudissa SN, Grieg MS, Hope C, Mahamed SH, Norbury R. 2016. Effect of chronotype on emotional processing and risk taking. Chronobiol Int. 33(4):406–418. doi:10.3109/07420528.2016.1146739.
  • Biss RK, Hasher L. 2012. Happy as a lark: morning-type younger and older adults are higher in positive affect. Emotion. 12(3):437–441. doi:10.1037/a0027071.
  • Blatter K, Cajochen C. 2007. Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physiol Behav. 90(2–3):196–208. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.009.
  • Bodenhausen GV. 1990. Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychol Sci. 1(5):319–322. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00226.x.
  • Boksem MAS, De Cremer D. 2010. Fairness concerns predict medial frontal negativity amplitude in ultimatum bargaining. Soc Neurosci. 5(1):118–128. doi:10.1080/17470910903202666.
  • Bradley MM, Lang PJ. 1994. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 25(1):49–59. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9.
  • Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. 1989. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28(2):193–213. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.
  • Caci H, Robert P, Boyer P. 2004. Novelty seekers and impulsive subjects are low in morningness. Eur Psychiatry J Assoc Eur Psychiatr. 19(2):79–84. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.09.007.
  • Camerer C. 2003. Behavioral Game theory: experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.
  • Castillo M, Dickinson DL, Petrie R. 2017. Sleepiness, choice consistency, and risk preferences. Theory Decis. 82(1):41–73. doi:10.1007/s11238-016-9559-7.
  • Choi S, Fisman R, Gale D, Kariv S. 2007. Consistency and heterogeneity of individual behavior under uncertainty. Am Econ Rev. 97(5):1921–1938.
  • Correa A, Lara T, Madrid JA. 2013. Influence of circadian typology and time of day on temporal preparation. Timing Time Percept. 1:217–238.
  • Correa A, Molina E, Sanabria D. 2014. Effects of chronotype and time of day on the vigilance decrement during simulated driving. Accid Anal Prev. 67:113–118. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2014.02.020.
  • Correa A, Ruiz-Herrera N, Ruz M, Tonetti L, Martoni M, Fabbri M, Natale V. 2017. Economic decision-making in morning/evening-type people as a function of time of day. Chronobiol Int. 34(2):139–147. doi:10.1080/07420528.2016.1246455.
  • Delorme A, Makeig S. 2004. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 134(1):9–21.
  • Díaz-Morales JF, Escribano C, Jankowski KS. 2015. Chronotype and time-of-day effects on mood during school day. Chronobiol Int. 32(1):37–42. doi:10.3109/07420528.2014.949736.
  • Dickinson DL, McElroy T. 2017. Sleep restriction and circadian effects on social decisions. Eur Econ Rev. 97:57–71. doi:10.1016/j.euroecorev.2017.05.002.
  • Dinges DF, Powell JW. 1985. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 17(6):652–655. doi:10.3758/BF03200977.
  • Dong G, Yang L. 2008. An ERP Study on the Process of Conflict Emotion Control. J Psychol Sci. 31:1365–1368.
  • Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR. 2002. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science. 295(5563):2279–2282. doi:10.1126/science.1066893.
  • Gino F, Ayal S, Ariely D. 2009. Contagion and differentiation in unethical behavior: the effect of one bad apple on the barrel. Psychol Sci. 20(3):393–398. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02306.x.
  • Gowen R, Filipowicz A, Ingram KK. 2019. Chronotype mediates gender differences in risk propensity and risk-taking. PLoS One. 14(5):e0216619. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0216619.
  • Gross JJ. 1998. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. 2(3):271–299. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271.
  • Gunia BC, Barnes CM, Sah S. 2014. The morality of larks and owls: unethical behavior depends on chronotype as well as time of day. Psychol Sci. 25(12):2272–2274. doi:10.1177/0956797614541989.
  • Güth W, Schmittberger R, Schwarze B. 1982. An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J Econ Behav Organ. 3(4):367–388. doi:10.1016/0167-2681(82)90011-7.
  • Horne CM, Norbury R. 2018. Late chronotype is associated with enhanced amygdala reactivity and reduced fronto-limbic functional connectivity to fearful versus happy facial expressions. NeuroImage. 171:355–363. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.025.
  • Horne JA, Brass CG, Pettitt AN. 1980. Ciradian performance differences between morning and evening “types”. Ergonomics. 23(1):29–36. doi:10.1080/00140138008924715.
  • Horne JA, Ostberg O. 1976. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 4(2):97–110.
  • Ingram KK, Ay A, Kwon SB, Woods K, Escobar S, Gordon M, Smith IH, Bearden N, Filipowicz A, Jain K. 2016. Molecular insights into chronotype and time-of-day effects on decision-making. Sci Rep. 6:29392. doi:10.1038/srep29392.
  • Jankowski KS. 2017. Social jet lag: sleep-corrected formula. Chronobiol Int. 34(4):531–535. doi:10.1080/07420528.2017.1299162.
  • Jennings JR, Wood CC. 1976. The epsilon-adjustment procedure for repeated-measure analyses of variance. Psychophysiology. 13:277–278.
  • Kerkhof GA. 1998. The 24-hour variation of mood differs between morning- and evening-type individuals. Percept Mot Skills. 86(1):264–266. doi:10.2466/pms.1998.86.1.264.
  • Kerkhof GA, Van Dongen HP. 1996. Morning-type and evening-type individuals differ in the phase position of their endogenous circadian oscillator. Neurosci Lett. 218(3):153–156.
  • Killgore WDS. 2007. Effects of sleep deprivation and morningness-eveningness traits on risk-taking. Psychol Rep. 100(2):613–626.
  • Killgore WDS, Balkin TJ, Wesensten NJ. 2006. Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 15(1):7–13. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00487.x.
  • Kleitman N. 1939. Sleep and wakefulness. Chicago: University of Chicago.
  • Kühnle T. 2006. Quantitative analysis of human chronotypes. Munich: Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat Munchen.
  • Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. 1999. International affective picture system (IAPS): technical manual and affective ratings. [place unknown], Gainesville: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
  • Lara T, Madrid JA, Correa A. 2014. The vigilance decrement in executive function is attenuated when individual chronotypes perform at their optimal time of day. PLoS One. 9(2):e88820. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0088820.
  • Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, Strong DR, Brown RA. 2002. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the balloon analogue risk task (BART). J Exp Psychol Appl. 8(2):75–84.
  • Levandovski R, Sasso E, Hidalgo MP. 2013. Chronotype: a review of the advances, limits and applicability of the main instruments used in the literature to assess human phenotype. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 35(1):3–11. doi:10.1590/S2237-60892013000100002.
  • Liu Y, Huang H, McGinnis-Deweese M, Keil A, Ding M. 2012. Neural substrate of the late positive potential in emotional processing. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 32(42):14563–14572. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3109-12.2012.
  • May CP, Hasher L. 1998. Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 24(2):363–379.
  • May CP, Hasher L, Foong N. 2005. Implicit memory, age, and time of day: paradoxical priming effects. Psychol Sci. 16(2):96–100.
  • Miller MA, Rothenberger SD, Hasler BP, Donofry SD, Wong PM, Manuck SB, Kamarck TW, Roecklein KA. 2015. Chronotype predicts positive affect rhythms measured by ecological momentary assessment. Chronobiol Int. 32(3):376–384. doi:10.3109/07420528.2014.983602.
  • Moser A, Gaertig C, Ruz M. 2014. Social information and personal interests modulate neural activity during economic decision-making. Front Hum Neurosci. 8:31. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00031.
  • Natale V. 1999. Validazione di una scala ridotta di Mattutinità (rMEQ). Boll Psicol Appl. 229:19–26.
  • Onton J, Westerfield M, Townsend J, Makeig S. 2006. Imaging human EEG dynamics using independent component analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 30(6):808–822. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.007.
  • Ortiz-Tudela E, Martinez-Nicolas A, Campos M, MÁ R, Madrid JA. 2010. A new integrated variable based on thermometry, actimetry and body position (TAP) to evaluate circadian system status in humans. PLoS Comput Biol. 6(11):e1000996. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000996.
  • Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, Echallier JF. 1989. Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 72(2):184–187.
  • Ponzi D, Wilson MC, Maestripieri D. 2014. Eveningness is associated with higher risk-taking, independent of sex and personality. Psychol Rep. 115(3):932–947. doi:10.2466/19.12.PR0.115c28z5.
  • Portaluppi F, Smolensky MH, Touitou Y. 2010. Ethics and methods for biological rhythm research on animals and human beings. Chronobiol Int. 27(9–10):1911–1929. doi:10.3109/07420528.2010.516381.
  • Porto R, Duarte L, Menna-Barreto L. 2006. Circadian variation of mood: comparison between different chronotypes. Biol Rhythm Res. 37(5):425–431. doi:10.1080/09291010600871477.
  • Potts GF, Martin LE, Burton P, Montague PR. 2006. When things are better or worse than expected: the medial frontal cortex and the allocation of processing resources. J Cogn Neurosci. 18(7):1112–1119. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1112.
  • Refinetti R. 1992. Non-parametric procedures for the determination of phase markers of circadian rhythms. Int J Biomed Comput. 30(1):49–56.
  • Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A, Merrow M. 2004. A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr Biol CB. 14(24):R1038–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.039.
  • Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. 2003. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms. 18(1):80–90.
  • Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. 2003. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science. 300(5626):1755–1758. doi:10.1126/science.1082976.
  • Sarabia JA, Rol MA, Mendiola P, Madrid JA. 2008. Circadian rhythm of wrist temperature in normal-living subjects A candidate of new index of the circadian system. Physiol Behav. 95(4):570–580. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.08.005.
  • Schmidt C, Collette F, Cajochen C, Peigneux P. 2007. A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn Neuropsychol. 24(7):755–789. doi:10.1080/02643290701754158.
  • Tonetti L, Adan A, Caci H, De Pascalis V, Fabbri M, Natale V. 2010. Morningness-eveningness preference and sensation seeking. Eur Psychiatry. 25(2):111–115. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.09.007.
  • Tversky A, Kahneman D. 1983. Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol Rev. 90(4):293–315. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293.
  • Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 54(6):1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.
  • Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. 2006. Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int. 23(1–2):497–509. doi:10.1080/07420520500545979.
  • Yoo -S-S, Gujar N, Hu P, Jolesz FA, Walker MP. 2007. The human emotional brain without sleep — a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr Biol. 17(20):R877–R878. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.007

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.