Publication Cover
Chronobiology International
The Journal of Biological and Medical Rhythm Research
Volume 38, 2021 - Issue 9
1,295
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

Time-of-day effects on objective and subjective short-term memory task performance

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 1330-1343 | Received 06 Aug 2020, Accepted 08 May 2021, Published online: 14 Jun 2021

References

  • Adan A, Archer SN, Hidalgo MP, Di Milia L, Natale V, Randler C. 2012. Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiol Int. 29(9):1153–1175. doi:10.3109/07420528.2012.719971
  • Alvaro PK, Roberts RM, Harris JK. 2014. The independent relationships between insomnia, depression, subtypes of anxiety, and chronotype during adolescence. Sleep Med. 15(8):934–941. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2014.03.019
  • Archer SN, Carpen JD, Gibson M, Lim GH, Johnston JD, Skene DJ, Schantz MV. 2010. Polymorphism in the PER3 promoter associates with diurnal preference and delayed sleep phase disorder. Sleep. 33(5):695–701. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.5.695
  • Archer SN, Schmidt C, Vandewalle G, Dijk DJ. 2018. Phenotyping of PER3 variants reveals widespread effects on circadian preference, sleep regulation, and health. Sleep Med Rev. 40:109–126. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2017.10.008
  • Atkins AS, Reuter-Lorenz PA. 2008. False working memories? Semantic distortion in a mere 4 seconds. Mem Cognit. 36(1):74–81. doi:10.3758/MC.36.1.74
  • Atkins AS, Reuter-Lorenz PA. 2011. Neural mechanisms of semantic interference and false recognition in short-term memory. NeuroImage. 56(3):1726–1734. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.048
  • Bailey M, Silver R. 2014. Sex differences in circadian timing systems: implications for disease. Front Neuroendocrinol. 35(1):111–139. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.11.003
  • Bell EC, Willson MC, Wilman AH, Dave S, Silverstone PH. 2006. Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks. NeuroImage. 30(2):529–538. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.049
  • Blessing W, Ootsuka Y. 2016. Timing of activities of daily life is jaggy: how episodic ultradian changes in body and brain temperature are integrated into this process. Temperature. 3(3):371–383. doi:10.1080/23328940.2016.1177159
  • Borbély AA. 1982. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1(3):195–204.
  • Borbély AA, Daan S, Wirz‐Justice A, Deboer T. 2016. The two‐process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J Sleep Res. 25(2):131–143. doi:10.1111/jsr.12371
  • Bosco A, Longoni AM, Vecchi T. 2004. Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental strategies. Appl Cogn Psychol. 18(5):519–532. doi:10.1002/acp.1000
  • Butler MP, Karatsoreos IN, LeSauter J, Silver R. 2012. Dose-dependent effects of androgens on the circadian timing system and its response to light. Endocrinology. 153(5):2344–2352. doi:10.1210/en.2011-1842
  • Carrier J, Monk TH. 2000. Circadian rhythms of performance: new trends. Chronobiol Int. 17(6):719–732. doi:10.1081/cbi-100102108
  • Corballis PM, Funnell MG, Gazzaniga MS. 1999. A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients. NeuroReport. 10(10):2183–2187. doi:10.1097/00001756-199907130-00033
  • Corballis PM, Funnell MG, Gazzaniga MS. 2002. Hemispheric asymmetries for simple visual judgments in the split brain. Neuropsychologia. 40(4):401–410. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00100-2
  • Corbera X, Grau C. 1993. Diurnal type and hemispheric asymmetry. Cortex. 29(3):519–528. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80257-2
  • Corbera X, Grau C, Vendrell P. 1993. Diurnal oscillations in hemispheric performance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 15(2):300–310. doi:10.1080/01688639308402564
  • Cowan N. 2008. What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Prog Brain Res. 169:323–338. doi:10.1016/s0079-6123(07)00020-9
  • Curby KM, Huang M, Moerel D. 2019. Multiple paths to holistic processing: holistic processing of Gestalt stimuli do not overlap with holistic face processing in the same manner as do objects of expertise. Atten Percept Psychophys. 81:716–726. doi:10.3758/s13414-018-01643-x
  • Deese J. 1959. On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. J Exp Psychol. 58(1):17–22. doi:10.1037/h0046671
  • Der G, Deary IJ. 2006. Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood: results from the United Kingdom health and lifestyle survey. Psychol Aging. 21(1):62–73. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.62
  • Díaz-Morales JF, Escribano C. 2013. Circadian preference and thinking styles: implications for school achievement. Chronobiol Int. 30(10):1231–1239. doi:10.3109/07420528.2013.813854
  • Dodson CS, Schacter DL. 2001. “If I had said it I would have remembered it: reducing false memories with a distinctiveness heuristic. Psychon Bull Rev. 8(1):155–161. doi:10.3758/BF03196152
  • Escribano C, Díaz‐Morales JF. 2016. Sleep habits and chronotype effects on academic and cognitive performance in spanish adolescents: a review. Int Online J Educ Sci. 8(5):17–29. doi:10.15345/iojes.2016.05.003
  • Evert DL, Kmen M. 2003. Hemispheric asymmetries for global and local processing as a function of stimulus exposure duration. Brain Cogn. 51(1):115–142. doi:10.1016/s0278-2626(02)00528-6
  • Fabbian F, Zucchi B, De Giorgi A, Tiseo R, Boari B, Salmi R, Cappadona R, Gianesini G, Bassi E, Signani F, et al. 2016. Chronotype, gender and general health. Chronobiol Int. 33(7):863–882. doi:10.1080/07420528.2016.1176927
  • Fabbri M, Antonietti A, Giorgetti M, Tonetti L, Natale V. 2007. Circadian typology and style of thinking differences. Learn Individ Differ. 17(2):175–180. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2007.05.002
  • Fabbri M, Mencarelli C, Adan A, Natale V. 2013. Time-of-day and circadian typology on memory retrieval. Biol Rhythm Res. 44(1):125–142. doi:10.1080/09291016.2012.656244
  • Fabbri M, Natale V, Adan A. 2008. Effect of time of day on arithmetic fact retrieval in a number-matching task. Acta Psychologica. 127(2):485–490. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.08.011
  • Fink GR, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ. 1996. Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? Nature. 382(6592):626–628. doi:10.1038/382626a0
  • Flevaris AV, Robertson LC. 2016. Spatial frequency selection and integration of global and local information in visual processing: a selective review and tribute to Shlomo Bentin. Neuropsychologia. 83:192–200. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.024
  • Folkard S. 1979. Time of day and level of processing. Mem Cognit. 7(4):247–252. doi:10.3758/BF03197596
  • Folkard S, Monk TH. 1983. Chronopsychology: circadian rhythms and human performance. In: Gale A, Edwards JA, editors. Physiological correlates of human behaviour: attention and performance (Vol. 11). London: Academic Press; p. 55–78.
  • Folkard S. 2008. Do permanent night workers show circadian adjustment? A review based on the endogenous melatonin rhythm. Chronobiol Int. 25(2–3):215–224. doi:10.1080/07420520802106835
  • Folkard S, Monk TH. 1980. Circadian rhythms in human memory. Br J Psychol. 71(2):295–307. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb01746.x
  • Folkard S, Wever RA, Wildgruber CM. 1983. Multi-oscillatory control of circadian rhythms in human performance. Nature. 305(5931):223–226. doi:10.1038/305223a0
  • Gallo DA, McDermott KB, Percer JM, Roediger HL. 2001. Modality effects in false recall and false recognition. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 27:339–353. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.2.339
  • Gazzaniga MS, Bogen JE, Sperry RW. 1965. Observations on visual perception after disconnexion of the cerebral hemispheres in man. Brain. 88(2):221–236. doi:10.1093/brain/88.2.221
  • Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Sebastiani T, Ottaviano S. 2002. Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behaviour in adolescence. J Sleep Res. 11(3):191–199. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00302.x
  • Goel N, Banks S, Mignot E, Dinges DF. 2009. PER3 polymorphism predicts cumulative sleep homeostatic but not neurobehavioral changes to chronic partial sleep deprivation. PLoS One. 4(6):e5874. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005874
  • Golby AJ, Poldrack RA, Brewer JB, Spencer D, Desmond JE, Aron AP, Gabrieli JDE. 2001. Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain. 124(9):1841–1854. doi:10.1093/brain/124.9.1841
  • González-Garrido AA, Gómez-Velázquez FR, Sequeira H, Ramos-Loyo J, López-Franco AL. 2013. Gender differences in visuospatial working memory-does emotion matter? Int J Psychol Stud. 5(1):11. doi:10.5539/ijps.v5n1p11
  • Green DM, Swets JA. 1966. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
  • Groeger JA, Viola AU, Lo JC, Von Schantz M, Archer SN, Dijk DJ. 2008. Early morning executive functioning during sleep deprivation is compromised by a PERIOD3 polymorphism. Sleep. 31(8):1159–1167. doi:10.5665/sleep/31.8.1159
  • Hahn N, Jansen P, Heil M. 2010. Preschoolers’ mental rotation: sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry. J Cogn Neurosci. 22(6):1244–1250. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21236
  • Han S, Liu W, Yund EW, Woods DL. 2000. Interactions between spatial attention and global/local feature selection. NeuroReport. 11(12):2753–2758. doi:10.1097/00001756-200008210-00029
  • Hasher L, Chung C, May CP, Foong N. 2002. Age, time of testing, and proactive interference. Can J Exp Psychol. 56(3):200. doi:10.1037/h0087397
  • Hornik J, Miniero G. 2009. Synchrony effects on customers’ responses and behaviors. Int J Res Mark. 26(1):34–40. doi:10.1016/j.ijresmar.2008.04.002
  • Huang J. 1993. An investigation of gender differences in cognitive abilities among Chinese high school students. Pers Individ Dif. 75:717–719. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90012-R
  • Hübner R, Volberg G. 2005. The integration of object levels and their content: a theory of global/local processing and related hemispheric differences. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 31(3):520–541. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.520
  • Hübner R, Volberg G, Studer T. 2007. Hemispheric differences for global/local processing in divided attention tasks: further evidence for the integration theory. Percept Psychophys. 69(3):413–421. doi:10.3758/bf03193762
  • Hyde JS. 2007. New directions in the study of gender similarities and differences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 16(5):259–263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00516.x
  • Iidaka T, Harada T, Sadato N. 2014. False memory for face in short-term memory and neural activity in human amygdala. Brain Res. 1591:74–85. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2014.10.003
  • Ingalhalikar M, Smith A, Parker D, Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Hakonarson H, Gur RE, Gur RC, Verma R. 2013. Sex differences in the structural connectome of the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 111(2):823–828. doi:10.1073/pnas.1316909110
  • Jain A, Bansal R, Kumar A, Singh KD. 2015. A comparative study of visual and auditory reaction times on the basis of gender and physical activity levels of medical first year students. Int J Appl Basic Med Res. 5(2):124–127. doi:10.4103/2229-516X.157168
  • Jiménez KM, Pereira-Morales AJ, Forero DA. 2017. Higher scores in the extraversion personality trait are associated with a functional polymorphism in the PER3 gene in healthy subjects. Chronobiol Int. 34(2):280–286. doi:10.1080/07420528.2016.1268149
  • Johns MW. 1991. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 14(6):540–545. doi:10.1093/sleep/14.6.540
  • Kuljis DA, Loh DH, Truong D, Vosko AM, Ong ML, McClusky R, Arnold AP, Colwell CS. 2013. Gonadal- and sex-chromosome-dependent sex differences in the circadian system. Endocrinology. 154(4):1501–1512. doi:10.1210/en.2012-1921
  • Kwon J, Kagitani T, Sakamoto M. 2017. Holistic processing affects surface texture perception: approach from Japanese sound symbolic words. J Cogn Sci (Seoul). 18(3):321–340. doi:10.17791/jcs.2017.18.3.321
  • Lack L, Bailey M, Lovato N, Wright H. 2009. Chronotype differences in circadian rhythms of temperature, melatonin, and sleepiness as measured in a modified constant routine protocol. Nat Sci Sleep. 1:1–8. doi:10.2147/nss.s6234
  • Lawton CA, Hatcher DW. 2005. Gender differences in integration of images in visuospatial memory. Sex Roles. 53(9–10):717–725. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-7736-1
  • Lewandowska K, Gagol A, Sikora-Wachowicz B, Marek T, Fafrowicz M. 2019. Saying “yes” when you want to say “no” - pupil dilation reflects evidence accumulation in a visual working memory recognition task. Int J Psychophysiol. 139:18–32. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.03.001Sikora-Wachowicz
  • Lewandowska K, Wachowicz B, Marek T, Oginska H, Fafrowicz M. 2018. Would you say “yes” in the evening? Time-of-day effect on response bias in four types of working memory recognition tasks. Chronobiol Int. 35(1):80–89. doi:10.1080/07420528.2017.1386666
  • Liberman AR, Kwon SB, Vu HT, Filipowicz A, Ay A, Ingram KK. 2017. Circadian clock model supports molecular link between PER3 and human anxiety. Sci Rep. 7(1):1–10. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-07957-4
  • Lowe PA, Mayfield JW, Reynolds CR. 2003. Gender differences in memory test performance among children and adolescents. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 18(8):865–878. doi:10.1093/arclin/18.8.865
  • Manber R, Baker FC, Gress JL. 2006. Sex differences in sleep and sleep disorders: a focus on women’s sleep. Int J Sleep Dis. 1:7–15.
  • Manly T, Lewis GH, Robertson IH, Watson PC, Datta AK. 2002. Coffee in the cornflakes: time-of-day as a modulator of executive response control. Neuropsychologia. 40(1):1–6. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00086-0
  • Martínez-Pérez V, Palmero LB, Campoy G, Fuentes LJ. 2020. The role of chronotype in the interaction between the alerting and the executive control networks. Sci Rep. 10(1):1. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-68755-z
  • May CP, Hasher L. 1998. Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 24(2):363. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.24.2.363
  • May CP, Hasher L, Foong N. 2005. Implicit memory, age, and time of day: paradoxical priming effects. Psychol Sci. 16(2):96–100. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00788.x
  • Merikanto I, Lahti T, Kronholm E, Peltonen M, Laatikainen T, Vartiainen E, Partonen T. 2013. Evening types are prone to depression. Chronobiol Int. 30(5):719–725. doi:10.3109/07420528.2013.784770
  • Milner B. 1971. Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Br Med Bull. 27(3):272–277. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070866
  • Mong JA, Baker FC, Mahoney MM, Paul KN, Schwartz MD, Semba K, Silver R. 2011. Sleep, rhythms, and the endocrine brain: influence of sex and gonadal hormones. J Neurosci. 31(45):16107–16116. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4175-11.2011
  • Mottron L, Soulières I. 2013. Global versus local processing. Encyclopedia of Autism spectrum disorders. New York, NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-1698-3
  • Natale V, Alzani A, Cicogna P. 2003. Cognitive efficiency and circadian typologies: a diurnal study. Pers Individ Dif. 35(5):1089–1105. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00320-3
  • Nikam LH, Gadkari JV. 2012. Effect of age, gender and body mass index on visual and auditory reaction times in Indian population. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 56(1):94–99.
  • Nowack K, Van Der Meer E. 2018. The synchrony effect revisited: chronotype, time of day and cognitive performance in a semantic analogy task. Chronobiol Int. 35(12):1647–1662. doi:10.1080/07420528.2018.1500477
  • Oginska H, Mojsa-Kaja J, Mairesse O. 2017. Chronotype description: in search of a solid subjective amplitude scale. Chronobiol Int. 34(10):1388–1400. doi:10.1080/07420528.2017.1372469
  • Owens DS, Macdonald I, Tucker P, Sytnik N, Minors D, Waterhouse J, Totterdell P, Folkard S. 1998. Diurnal trends in mood and performance do not all parallel alertness. Scand J Work Environ Health. 24(3):109–114.
  • Petersen J. 2018. Gender difference in verbal performance: a meta-analysis of United States State performance assessments. Educ Psychol Rev. 30(4):02. doi:10.1007/s10648-018-9450-x
  • Portaluppi F, Smolensky MH, Touitou Y. 2010. Ethics and methods for biological rhythm research on animals and human beings. Chronobiol Int. 27(9–10):1911–1929. doi:10.3109/07420528.2010.516381
  • Ramírez C, Talamantes J, García A, Morales M, Valdez P, Menna-Barreto L. 2006. Circadian rhythms in phonological and visuospatial storage components of working memory. Biol Rhythm Res. 37(5):433–441. doi:10.1080/09291010600870404
  • Reinke L, Özbay Y, Dieperink W, Tulleken JE. 2015. The effect of chronotype on sleepiness, fatigue, and psychomotor vigilance of ICU nurses during the night shift. Intensive Care Med. 41(4):657–666. doi:10.1007/s00134-015-3667-7
  • Richardson JTE. 2011. Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educ Res Rev. 6(2):135–147. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2010.12.001
  • Robertson LC, Lamb MR, Knight RT. 1988. Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. J Neurosci. 8(10):3757–3769. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-10-03757.1988
  • Roediger HL, McDermott KB. 1995. Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 21(4):803–814. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803
  • Roenneberg T, Allebrandt KV, Merrow M, Vetter C. 2012. Social jetlag and obesity. Curr Biol. 22(10):939–943. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.038
  • Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A, Merrow M. 2004. A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr Biol. 14(24):1038–1039. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.039
  • Roenneberg T, Merrow M. 2005. Circadian clocks—the fall and rise of physiology. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol. 6(12):965–971. doi:10.1038/nrm1766
  • Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. 2003. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms. 18(1):80–90. doi:10.1177/0748730402239679
  • Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. 2009. Short article: age and synchrony effects in visuospatial working memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 62(10):1873–1880. doi:10.1080/17470210902834852
  • Santhi N, Lazar AS, McCabe PJ, Lo JC, Groeger JA, Dijk D-J. 2016. Sex differences in the circadian regulation of sleep and waking cognition in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 113(19):2730–2739. doi:10.1073/pnas.1521637113
  • Schmidt C, Collette F, Cajochen C, Peigneux P. 2007. A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn Neuropsychol. 24(7):755–789. doi:10.1080/02643290701754158
  • Schmidt C, Collette F, Reichert CF, Maire M, Vandewalle G, Peigneux P, Cajochen C. 2015. Pushing the limits: chronotype and time of day modulate working memory-dependent cerebral activity. Front Neurol. 6:199. doi:10.3389/fneur.2015.00199
  • Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Leclercq Y, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Phillips C, Berthomier P, Berthomier C, Tinguely G, Gais S, et al. 2012. Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS One. 7(1):e29658. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029658
  • Schöning S, Engelien A, Kugel H, Schäfer S, Schiffbauer H, Zwitserlood P, Pletziger E, Beizai P, Kersting A, Ohrmann P. 2007. Functional anatomy of visuo-spatial working memory during mental rotation is influenced by sex, menstrual cycle, and sex steroid hormones. Neuropsychologia. 45(14):3203–3214. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.011
  • Sikora-Wachowicz B, Keresztes A, Werkle-Bergner M, Lewandowska K, Marek T, Fafrowicz M. 2021. False recognitions in short-term memory: age-differences in neural activity. Brain Cognit. 151:1–15. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105728
  • Sikora-Wachowicz B, Lewandowska K, Keresztes A, Werkle-Bergner M, Marek T, Fafrowicz M. 2019. False recognition in short-term memory – age-differences in confidence. Front Psychol. 10:2785. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02785
  • Slotnick SD, Schacter DL. 2004. A sensory signature that distinguishes true from false memories. Nat Neurosci. 7(6):664–672. doi:10.1038/nn1252
  • Smith R, Hunt E. 1998. Presentation modality affects false memory. Psychon Bull Rev. 5(4):710–715. doi:10.3758/BF03208850
  • Speck O, Ernst T, Braun J, Koch C, Miller E, Chang L. 2000. Gender differences in the functional organization of the brain for working memory. NeuroReport. 11(11):2581–2585. doi:10.1097/00001756-200008030-00046
  • Sternberg RJ. 1997. Thinking styles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Tassi P, Pellerin N, Moessinger M, Eschenlauer R, Muzet A. 2012. Variation of visual detection over the 24-hour period in humans. Chronobiol Int. 17(6):795–805. doi:10.1081/cbi-100102115
  • Thompson AE, Voyer D. 2014. Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: a meta-analysis. Cogn Emot. 28(7):1164–1195. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.875889
  • Tian L, Wang J, Yan C, He Y. 2011. Hemisphere- and gender-related differences in small-world brain networks: a resting-state functional MRI study. NeuroImage. 54(1):191–202. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.066
  • Unsworth N, Engle RW. 2007. On the division of short-term and working memory: an examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychol Bull. 133(6):1038–1066. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1038
  • Urbán R, Magyaródi T, Rigó A. 2011. Morningness-eveningness, chronotypes and health-impairing behaviors in adolescents. Chronobiol Int. 28(3):238–247. doi:10.3109/07420528.2010.549599
  • Valdez P. 2018. Homeostatic and circadian regulation of cognitive performance. Biol Rhythm Res. 50(1):85–93. doi:10.1080/09291016.2018.1491271
  • Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A. 2012. Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: implications for neuropsychological assessment. Chronophysiol Ther. 2:81–92. doi:10.2147/cpt.s32586
  • Vandewalle G, Archer SN, Wuillaume C, Balteau E, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Maquet P, Dijk DJ. 2009. Functional magnetic resonance imaging-assessed brain responses during an executive task depend on interaction of sleep homeostasis, circadian phase, and PER3 genotype. J Neurosci. 29(25):7948–7956. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0229-09.2009
  • Vandewalle G, Gais S, Schabus M, Balteau E, Carrier J, Darsaud A, Sterpenich V, Albouy G, Dijk DJ, Maquet P. 2007. Wavelength-dependent modulation of brain responses to a working memory task by daytime light exposure. Cereb Cortex. 17(12):2788–2795. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm007
  • Viola AU, Archer SN, James LM, Groeger JA, Lo JC, Skene DJ, Von Schantz M, Dijk DJ. 2007. PER3 polymorphism predicts sleep structure and waking performance. Curr Biol. 17(7):613–618. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.073
  • Viola AU, Chellappa SL, Archer SN, Pugin F, Götz T, Dijk DJ, Cajochen C. 2012. Interindividual differences in circadian rhythmicity and sleep homeostasis in older people: effect of a PER3 polymorphism. Neurobiol Aging. 33(5):1010–e17. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.10.024
  • West R, Murphy KJ, Armilio ML, Craik FI, Stuss DT. 2002. Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 57(1):3–10. doi:10.1093/geronb/57.1.P3
  • Wieth M, Zacks R. 2011. Time of day effects on problem solving: when the non-optimal is optimal. Think Reason. 17(4):387–401. doi:10.1080/13546783.2011.625663
  • Yang L, Hasher L, Wilson DE. 2007. Synchrony effects in automatic and controlled retrieval. Psychon Bull Rev. 14(1):51–56. doi:10.3758/BF03194027
  • Zhao M, Bülthoff HH, Bülthoff I. 2015. Beyond Faces and Expertise. Psychol Sci. 27(2):213–222. doi:10.1177/0956797615617779
  • Zuanazzi A, Cattaneo L. 2017. The right hemisphere is independent from the left hemisphere in allocating visuospatial attention. Neuropsychologia. 102:197–205. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.005

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.