Publication Cover
Child Neuropsychology
A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence
Latest Articles
33
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The prediction of emotional decision making from working memory and inhibitory control in preschool children: using decision tree model

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Received 17 May 2023, Accepted 06 Jun 2024, Published online: 19 Jun 2024

References

  • Achenback, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families.
  • Allan, N. P., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Examining the dimensionality of effortful control in preschool children and its relation to academic and socioemotional indicators. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 905–915. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023748
  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Kirkwood, H., & Elliott, J. (2008). Evaluating the validity of the automated working memory assessment. Educational Psychology, 28(7), 725–734. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410802243828
  • Anderson, V. (1998). Assessing Executive functions in children: Biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8(3), 319–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755568
  • Anderson, P. J., & Reidy, N. (2012). Assessing executive function in preschoolers. Neuropsychology Review, 22(4), 345–360. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-9220-3
  • Apté, C., & Weiss, S. (1997). Data mining with decision trees and decision rules. Future Generation Computer Systems, 13(2–3), 197–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-739x(97)00021-6
  • Audusseau, J., & Juhel, J. (2015). Working memory in children predicts performance on a gambling task. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 176(1–2), 38–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2014.1002749
  • Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11(1), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1009085417776
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3), 7–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3
  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain A Journal of Neurology, 123(11), 2189–2202. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.11.2189
  • Benton, A. L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6(1), 53–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(68)90038-9
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
  • Blair, C. (2016). Developmental science and executive function. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(1), 3–7. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721415622634
  • Bodnar, L. E., Prahme, M. C., Cutting, L. E., Denckla, M. B., & Mahone, E. M. (2007). Construct validity of parent ratings of inhibitory control. Child Neuropsychology, 13(4), 345–362. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040600899867
  • Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., & Grimm, K. J. (2009a). The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24(3), 337–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.06.001
  • Buelow, M. T., & Suhr, J. A. (2009). Construct validity of the Iowa gambling task. Neuropsychology Review, 19(1), 102–114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9083-4
  • Bunch, K. M., & Andrews, G. (2012). Development of relational processing in hot and cool tasks. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(2), 134–152. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2011.632457
  • Byrnes, J. P. (2002). The development of decision-making. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 31(6 Suppl), 208–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00503-7
  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333
  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.298
  • Crone, E. A., Bunge, S. A., Latenstein, H., & van der Molen, M. W. (2005). Characterization of children’s decision making: Sensitivity to punishment frequency, not task complexity. Child Neuropsychology, 11(3), 245–263. https://doi.org/10.1080/092970490911261
  • Crone, E. A., & van der Molen, M. W. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision making: Performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(3), 251–279. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2503_2
  • Delgado, H., & Carboni, A. (2022). The role of local violence on children’s affective decision-making. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-06908-6_7
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
  • Diamond, A. (2020). Executive functions. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 173, 225–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64150-2.00020-4
  • Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking Executive function and its development. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 15(4), 942–956. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620904771
  • Dowsett, S. M., & Livesey, D. J. (2000). The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: Effects of “executive skills” training. Developmental Psychobiology, 36(2), 161–174. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(200003)36:2<161:AID-DEV7>3.0.CO;2-0
  • Dretsch, M. N., & Tipples, J. (2008). Working memory involved in predicting future outcomes based on past experiences. Brain & Cognition, 66(1), 83–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.05.006
  • Fokkema, M., Iliescu, D., Greiff, S., & Ziegler, M. (2022). Machine learning and prediction in psychological assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 38(3), 165–175. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000714
  • Gao, S., Wei, Y., Bai, J., Lin, C., & Li, H. (2009). Young children’s affective decision-making in a gambling task: Does difficulty in learning the gain/loss schedule matter? Cognitive Development, 24(2), 183–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.07.006
  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
  • Garon, N., Hecker, O., Kwan, A., Crocker, T. A., & English, S. D. (2023). Integrated versus trial specific focus improves decision-making in older preschoolers. Child Neuropsychology, 29(1), 28–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2022.2063269
  • Garon, N., & Moore, C. (2004). Complex decision-making in early childhood. Brain Cogn, 55(1), 158–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00272-0
  • Garon, N., & Moore, C. (2007). Awareness and symbol use improves future-oriented decision making in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31(1), 39–59. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn3101_3
  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177–190. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177
  • Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/2–7 years old on a stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-x
  • Heilman, R. M., Miu, A. C., & Benga, O. (2009). Developmental and sex-related differences in preschoolers’ affective decision making. Child Neuropsychology, 15(1), 73–84. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040802266436
  • Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2002). Somatic markers, working memory, and decision making. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(4), 341–353. https://doi.org/10.3758/cabn.2.4.341
  • Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(2), 298–306. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298
  • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 617–644. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4
  • Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The children’s gambling task. Brain Cogn, 55(1), 148–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00275-6
  • Lagattuta, K. H., Sayfan, L., & Monsour, M. (2011). A new measure for assessing executive function across a wide age range: Children and adults find happy-sad more difficult than day-night. Developmental Science, 14(3), 481–489. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00994.x
  • Lee, S. W., Guo, N. W., Huang, C. C., Huang, P. C., Chiang, C. J., & Chien, Y. H. (2022). Development of cool and hot executive function deficit in children born very low birth weight with normal early development: A longitudinal cohort from aged 6 to 10. Early Human Development, 175, 105693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2022.105693
  • Manes, F., Sahakian, B., Clark, L., Rogers, R., Antoun, N., Aitken, M., & Robbins, T. (2002). Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. Brain A Journal of Neurology, 125(Pt 3), 624–639. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf049
  • Mata, F., Sallum, I., Miranda, D. M., Bechara, A., & Malloy-Diniz, L. F. (2013). Do general intellectual functioning and socioeconomic status account for performance on the Children’s gambling task? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 68. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00068
  • Mattei, T. A. (2014). Unveiling complexity: Non-linear and fractal analysis in neuroscience and cognitive psychology. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 8, 17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2014.00017
  • Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.106.1.3
  • Meuwissen, A. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2014). Hot and cool executive function: Foundations for learning and healthy development. Zero to Three, 35, 18–23.
  • Montroy, J. J., Merz, E. C., Williams, J. M., Landry, S. H., Johnson, U. Y., Zucker, T. A. Assel, M., Taylor, H. B., Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., Clancy-Menchetti, & J. de Villiers, P. (2019). Hot and cool dimensionality of executive function: Model invariance across age and maternal education in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 49, 188–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.06.011
  • Moriguchi, Y., & Phillips, S. (2023). Evaluating the distinction between cool and hot executive function during childhood. Brain Sciences, 13(2), 313. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020313
  • Noel, X., Bechara, A., Dan, B., Hanak, C., & Verbanck, P. (2007). Response inhibition deficit is involved in poor decision making under risk in nonamnesic individuals with alcoholism. Neuropsychology, 21(6), 778–786. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.778
  • O’Toole, S. E., Monks, C. P., & Tsermentseli, S. (2017). Executive function and theory of mind as predictors of aggressive and prosocial behavior and peer acceptance in early childhood. Social Development, 26(4), 907–920. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12231
  • Pecchinenda, A., Dretsch, M., & Chapman, P. (2006). Working memory involvement in emotion-based processes underlying choosing advantageously. Experimental Psychology, 53(3), 191–197. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.53.3.191
  • Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 621–637. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008
  • Salzberg, S. L. (1994). Programs for machine learning by J. Ross Quinlan Morgan Kaufmann Publishers, 1993. Machine Learning, 16(3), 235–240. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00993309
  • Wechsler, D. (1991). WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence scale for children: Manual. Psychological Corporation.
  • Weiland, C., Barata, M. C., & Yoshikawa, H. (2014). The Co-occurring development of Executive function skills and receptive vocabulary in preschool-aged children: A look at the direction of the developmental pathways. Infant and Child Development, 23(1), 4–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1829
  • Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Developmental Psychobiology, 44(1), 68–83. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.10152
  • Xiao, L., Bechara, A., Palmer, P. H., Trinidad, D. R., Wei, Y., Jia, Y., & Johnson, C. A. (2011). Parent-Child engagement in decision making and the development of adolescent affective decision capacity and binge drinking. Pers Individ Dif, 51(3), 285–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.04.023
  • Yarkoni, T., & Westfall, J. (2017). Choosing prediction over explanation in psychology: Lessons from machine learning. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 12(6), 1100–1122. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617693393
  • Zelazo, P. D. (2020). Executive function and psychopathology: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 431–454. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072319-024242
  • Zelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner-Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L., & Weintraub, S. (2013). II. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 16–33. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12032
  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, n/a-n/a. 6(4), 354–360. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x
  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2020). The neurodevelopment of executive function skills: Implications for academic achievement gaps. Psychology & Neuroscience, 13(3), 273–298. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000208
  • Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive functions in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook ofchildhood cognitive development (pp. 574–603). Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444325485.ch22

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.