788
Views
21
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Memory systems and the control of skilled action

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 692-718 | Received 13 Mar 2018, Accepted 18 Jan 2019, Published online: 23 May 2019

References

  • Anderson, J. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89(4), 369–406.
  • Behnke, E. (1997). Ghost gestures: Phenomenological investigations of bodily micromovements and their intercorporeal implications. Human Studies, 20, 181–201.
  • Bicknell, K. (2010). Feeling them ride: Corporeal exchange in cross-country mountain bike racing. About Performance, 10, 81–91.
  • Botvinick, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2002). Representing task context: Proposals based on a connectionist model of action. Psychological Research, 66(4), 298–311.
  • Botvinick, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111(2), 395–429.
  • Botvinick, M. M. (2008). Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 201–208.
  • Brown, R. M., Zatorre, R. J., & Penhune, V. B. (2015). Expert music performance: Cognitive, neural, and developmental bases. In E. Altenmüller, S. Finger, & F. Boller (Eds.), Progress in brain research (Vol. 217, pp. 57–86). Oxford: Elsevier.
  • Bryan, W. L., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological Review, 6(4), 345–375.
  • Butterfill, S. A., & Sinigaglia, C. (2014). Intention and motor representation in purposive action. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 88(1), 119–145.
  • Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Ideomotor apraxia: A call to action. Neurocase, 7(6), 445–458.
  • Carson, H. J., & Collins, D. (2011). Refining and regaining skills in fixation/diversification stage performers: The five-A model. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 4(2), 146–167.
  • Chaffin, R., & Logan, T. (2006). Practicing perfection: How concert soloists prepare for performance. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2(2–3), 113–130.
  • Christensen, W., Bicknell, K., McIlwain, D., & Sutton, J. (2015). The sense of agency and its role in strategic control for expert mountain bikers. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2(3), 340–353.
  • Christensen, W., Sutton, J., & McIlwain, D. J. F. (2016). Cognition in skilled action: Meshed control and the varieties of skill experience. Mind & Language, 31(1), 37–66.
  • Christensen, W. D., & Bicknell, K. (2018). Affordances and the anticipatory control of action. In M. Cappuccio (Ed.), The MIT press handbook of embodied cognition and sport psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Christensen, W. D., Sutton, J., & Bicknell, K. (in preparation). Meshed control in skilled action.
  • Clark, D., & Ivry, R. B. (2010). Multiple systems for motor skill learning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(4), 461–467.
  • Cooper, R., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(4), 297–338.
  • Curran, T., & Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(1), 189–202.
  • Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1–2), 1–37.
  • Dreyfus, H. L. (2013). The myth of the pervasiveness of the mental. In J. K. Schear (Ed.), Mind, reason, and being-in-the-world (pp. 15–38). London: Routledge.
  • Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (1986). Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. New York: Free Press.
  • Dreyfus, S. E. (2014). System 0: The overlooked explanation of expert intuition. In M. Sinclair (Ed.), Handbook of research methods on intuition (pp. 15–27). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishers.
  • Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1(1), 41–50.
  • Eichenbaum, H., Stewart, C., & Morris, R. G. (1990). Hippocampal representation in place learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10(11), 3531–3542.
  • Ennen, E. (2003). Phenomenological coping skills and the striatal memory system. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 299–325.
  • Ericsson, K. A. (2006a). Protocol analysis and expert thought: Concurrent verbalizations of thinking during experts’ performance on representative tasks. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, R. R. Hoffman, & P. J. Feltovich (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 223–242). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Ericsson, K. A. (2006b). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the development of superior expert performance. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, R. R. Hoffman, & P. J. Feltovich (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 683–704). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102(2), 211–244.
  • Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence and maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47(1), 273–305.
  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.
  • Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
  • Fridland, E. (2014). They’ve lost control: Reflections on skill. Synthese, 191(12), 2729–2750.
  • Goldenberg, G. (2013). Apraxia: The cognitive side of motor control. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking, and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10(1), 42–54.
  • Hommel, B. (2007). Consciousness and Control: Not Identical Twins. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(1–2), 155–176.
  • Huys, R., Daffert-Shofer, A., & Beek, P. J. (2004). The evolution of coordination during skill acquisition: The dynamical systems approach. In A. M. Williams, N. J. Hodges, & M. Scott (Eds.), Skill Acquisition in Sport: Research, Theory and Practice (pp. 351–373). New York: Routledge.
  • Jiménez, L., & Méndez, C. (1999). Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 236–259.
  • Jiménez, L., & Méndez, C. (2001). Implicit sequence learning with competing explicit cues. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 54(2), 345–369.
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110(2), 316–339.
  • Keisler, A., & Shadmehr, R. (2010). A shared resource between declarative memory and motor memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(44), 14817–14823.
  • Kelso, J. A. S., Holt, K. G., Rubin, P., & Kugler, P. N. (1981). Patterns of human interlimb coordination emerge from the properties of non-linear, limit-cycle, oscillatory processes: Theory and data. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 13, 226–261.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 635–642.
  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–131). New York: Wiley.
  • Leavitt, J. L. (1979). Cognitive demands of skating and stickhandling in ice hockey. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences. Journal Canadien Des Sciences Appliquées Au Sport, 4(1), 46–55.
  • Logan, S. W., & Fischman, M. G. (2011). The relationship between end-state comfort effects and memory performance in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 137(3), 292–299.
  • Logan, S. W., & Fischman, M. G. (2015). The death of recency: Relationship between end-state comfort and serial position effects in serial recall: Logan and Fischman (2011) revisited. Human Movement Science, 44, 11–21.
  • Margulis, E. H. (2005). A model of melodic expectation. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22(4), 663–714.
  • McIlwain, D., & Sutton, J. (2014). Yoga from the mat up: How words alight on bodies. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 46(6), 655–673.
  • McPherson, S. L., & Vickers, J. N. (2004). Cognitive control in motor expertise. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 2(3), 274–300.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46(3), 774–785.
  • Milner, B. (1962). Les troubles de la memoire accompagnant des lesions hippocampiques bilaterales. In Physiologie de l’hippocampe (pp. 257–272). Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.
  • Milner, D., & Goodale, M. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Montero, B. (2010). Does bodily awareness interfere with highly skilled movement? Inquiry, 53(2), 105–122.
  • Montero, B. (2013). A dancer reflects. In J. K. Schear (Ed.), Mind, reason, and being-in-the-world (pp. 303–319). London: Routledge.
  • Montero, B. G. (2016). Thought in action: expertise and the conscious mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Mylopoulos, M., & Pacherie, E. (2017). Intentions and motor representations: The interface challenge. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 8(2), 317–336.
  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research, Vol. IV (pp. 1–18). New York: Springer.
  • Ochipa, C., Rothi, L. J. G., & Heilman, K. M. (1989). Ideational apraxia: A deficit in tool selection and use. Annals of Neurology, 25(2), 190–193.
  • Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition, 107(1), 179–217.
  • Packard, M. G., & McGaugh, J. L. (1996). Inactivation of hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 65(1), 65–72.
  • Palmer, C., & Meyer, R. K. (2000). Conceptual and motor learning in music performance. Psychological Science, 11(1), 63–68.
  • Papineau, D. (2013). In the zone. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements, 73, 175–196.
  • Pavese, C. (2015). Practical Senses. Philosophers’ Imprint, 50(29), 1–25.
  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Carlson, R. A., & Gilmore, R. O. (2001). Acquisition of intellectual and perceptual-motor skills. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 453–470.
  • Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8(6), 443–458.
  • Rubin, D. C. (2016). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(4), 277–311. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00017.x
  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82(4), 225–260.
  • Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (2008). Motor learning and performance: A situation-based learning approach. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  • Schneider, W., & Chein, J. M. (2003). Controlled and automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 27(3), 525–559.
  • Shepherd, J. (2015). Conscious control over action. Mind & Language, 30(3), 320–344.
  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190.
  • Sohn, Y. W., & Doane, S. M. (2004). Memory processes of flight situation awareness: Interactive roles of working memory capacity, long-term working memory, and expertise. Human Factors: the Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 46(3), 461–475.
  • Spiegel, M. A., Koester, & Schack, T. (2013). The functional role of working memory in the (re-)planning and execution of grasping movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1326–1339.
  • Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82(3), 171–177.
  • Squire, L. R. (2009). Memory and brain systems: 1969–2009. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(41), 12711–12716.
  • Stanley, J., & Krakauer, J. W. (2013). Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(503), 1–11.
  • Steinerberger, S. (2015). On the number of positions in chess without promotion. International Journal of Game Theory, 44(3), 761–767.
  • Sutton, J. (2007). Batting, habit and memory: The embodied mind and the nature of skill. Sport in Society, 10(5), 763–786.
  • Sutton, J., & McIlwain, D. (2015). Breadth and depth of knowledge in expert versus novice athletes. In D. Farrow & J. Baker (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of sport expertise (pp. 95–105). London: Routledge.
  • Sutton, J. (2015). Remembering as public practice: Wittgenstein, memory, and distributed cognitive ecologies. In D. Moyal-Sharrock, A. Coliva, & V. Munz (Eds.), Mind, language, and action (pp. 409–443). Walter de Gruyter.
  • Sutton, J., McIlwain, D., Christensen, W. D., & Geeves, A. (2011). Applying intelligence to the reflexes: Embodied skills and habits between Dreyfus and Descartes. Journal of the British Society for Phenomenology, 42(1), 78–103.
  • Toner, J., Montero, B. G., & Moran, A. (2015). Considering the role of cognitive control in expert performance. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 1–18.
  • Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94(1), 3–15.
  • van Gaal, S., de Lange, F. P., & Cohen, M. X. (2012). The role of consciousness in cognitive control and decision making. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(121), 1–15.
  • Weigelt, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., Huelshorst, S., & Schack, T. (2009). Moving and memorizing: Motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 132(1), 68–79.
  • Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11(7–8), 1317–1329.
  • Yarrow, K., Brown, P., & Krakauer, J. W. (2009). Inside the brain of an elite athlete: The neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(8), 585–596.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.