144
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Type-specific proactive interference in patients with semantic and phonological STM deficits

, &
Pages 972-989 | Received 07 Jan 2013, Accepted 23 Oct 2013, Published online: 03 Dec 2013

REFERENCES

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983) A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261–295. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90201-3
  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical data base on CD-ROM. Philadephia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium.
  • Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586–595.
  • Barde, L. H. F., Schwartz, M. F., Chrysikou, E. G., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2010). Reduced short-term memory span in aphasia and susceptibility to interference: Contribution of material-specific maintenance deficits. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4, 909–920. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.010
  • Barde, L. H. F., Schwartz, M. F., & Thompson-Schill, S. (2006). The role of left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) in semantic short-term memory: A comparison of two case studies. Brain and Language, 99, 71–72. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.050
  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 497–505.
  • Cowan, N. (2003) Varieties of procedural accounts of working memory retention systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(6), 731–732. doi:10.1017/S0140525X03250160
  • Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1196–1208. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00224-X
  • Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2005). Testing for suspected impairments and dissociations in single-case studies in neuropsychology: Evaluation of alternatives using Monte Carlo simulations and revised tests for dissociations. Neuropsychology, 19, 318–331. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.318
  • Davis, C. J. (2005). N-Watch: A program for deriving neighbourhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics. Behavior Research Methods, 37(1), 65–70.
  • Dell, G., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E., & Gagnon, D. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104, 801–838. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.801
  • Freedman, M., & Martin, R. (2001). Dissociable components of short-term memory and their relation to long-term learning. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 193–226.
  • Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2002). Inhibition and proactive interference effects in impaired semantic short-term memory of aphasic patients. Brain and Language, 83, 112–123.
  • Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2007). Proactive interference in a semantic short term memory deficit: Role of semantic and phonological relatedness. Cortex, 43, 112–123. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70449-0
  • Harris, L., Olson, A., & Humphreys, G. (submitted). The link between STM and sentence comprehension: A neuropsychological rehabilitation study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation.
  • Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Explaining semantic short-term memory deficits: Evidence for the critical role of semantic control. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 368–381.
  • Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1992) Pyramids and Palm Trees: A test of semantic access from words and pictures. Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company.
  • Jefferies, E., Hoffman, P., Jones, R., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2008). The impact of semantic impairment on verbal short-term memory in stroke aphasia and semantic dementia: A comparative study. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(1), 66–87.
  • Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Semantic impairment in stroke aphasia versus semantic dementia: A case-series comparison. Brain, 129, 2132–2147. doi:10.1093/brain/awl153
  • Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R. A., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 8410–8413. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.14.8410
  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2000). Working memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 336–358. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.2.336
  • Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). PALPA: Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.
  • Lustig, C., May, C., & Hasher, L. (2001). Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130(2), 199–207. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.199
  • Martin, R. C., & He, T. (2004). Semantic short-term memory and its role in sentence processing: A replication. Brain and Language, 89(1), 76–82.
  • Martin, R. C., & Romani, C. (1994). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension: A multiple components view. Neuropsychology, 8, 506–523. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.506
  • Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. R., & Yaffee, L. S., (1994). Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 83–111. doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1005
  • May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Kane, M. J. (1999). The role of interference in memory span. Memory and Cognition, 27, 759–767. doi:10.3758/BF03198529
  • Monsell, S. (1978). Recency, immediate recognition memory, and reaction time. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 465–501. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(78)90008-7
  • Nelson, J. K., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Persson, J., Sylvester, C.-Y. C., & Jonides, J. (2009). Mapping interference resolution across task domains: A shared control process in left inferior frontal gyrus. Brain Research, 1256, 92–100. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.001
  • Ruchkin, D. S., Grafman, J., Cameron, K., & Berndt, R. S. (2003). Working memory retention systems: A state of activated long-term memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 709–728
  • Shimamura, A. P., Jurica, P. J., Mangels, J. A., & Knight, R. T. (1995). Susceptibility to memory interference effects following frontal lobe damage: Findings from tests of paired-associate learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(2), 144–152. doi:10.1162/jocn.1995.7.2.144
  • Thompson-Schill, S. L., D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., & Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: A re-evaluation. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, USA. Neurobiology, 94, 14792–14797.
  • Wilson, M. D. (1988). The MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine readable dictionary, version 2. Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 20(1), 6–11.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.