1,325
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Animacy effects extend to working memory: results from serial order recall tasks

, ORCID Icon &
Pages 157-171 | Received 08 Apr 2019, Accepted 22 Nov 2019, Published online: 10 Dec 2019

References

  • Andrews, G. (2010). Belief-based and analytic processing in transitive inference depends on premise integration difficulty. Memory & Cognition, 38, 928–940. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.7.928
  • Andrews, G., Birney, D., & Halford, G. S. (2006). Relational processing and working memory capacity in comprehension of relative clause sentences. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1325–1340. doi: 10.3758/bf03193275
  • Andrews, G., Ogden, J. E., & Halford, G. S. (2017). Resolving conflicts between syntax and plausibility in sentence comprehension. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 13, 11–27. doi: 10.5709/acp-0203-8
  • Aslan, A., & John, T. (2016). The development of adaptive memory: Young children show enhanced retention of animacy-related information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 343–350. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.007
  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (pp. 47–90). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589.
  • Bailey, H., Dunlosky, J., & Kane, M. J. (2011). Contribution of strategy use to performance on complex and simple span tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39, 447–461. doi: 10.3758/s13421-010-0034-3
  • Barrett, H. C. (2005). Adaptions to predators and prey. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 220–233). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
  • Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 83–100. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.83
  • Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2014). On the proper reading of the TBRS model: Reply to Oberauer and Lewandowsky (2014). Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1331–1334. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01331
  • Barrouillet, P., Portrat, S., & Camos, V. (2011). On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. Psychological Review, 118, 175–192. doi: 10.1037/a0022324
  • Bayliss, D. M., Bogdanovs, J., & Jarrold, C. (2015). Consolidating working memory: Distinguishing the effects of consolidation, rehearsal and attentional refreshing in a working memory span task. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 34–50. doi: 10.106/j.ml.2014.12.004
  • Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Gunn, D. M. (2007). The relationship between short-term memory and working memory: Complex span made simple? Memory (Hove, England), 13, 414–421. doi: 10.1080/09658210344000332
  • Bird, H., Franklin, S., & Howard, D. (2001). Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 33, 73–79. doi: 10.3758/BF03195349
  • Bonin, P., Gelin, M., & Bugaiska, A. (2014). Animates are better remembered than inanimates: Further evidence from word and picture stimuli. Memory & Cognition, 42, 370–382. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0368-8
  • Bonin, P., Gelin, M., Laroche, B., Meot, A., & Bugaiska, A. (2015). The “how” of animacy effects in episodic memory. Experimental Psychology, 62, 371–384. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000308
  • Bugaiska, A., Gregoire, L., Camblats, A. M., Gelin, M., Meot, A., & Bonin, P. (2019). Animacy and attentional processes: Evidence from the Stroop task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 882–889. doi: 10.1177/1747021818771514
  • Bugaiska, A., Meot, A., & Bonin, P. (2016). Do healthy elders, like young adults, remember animates better than inanimates? An adaptive view. Experimental Aging Research, 42, 447–459. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2016.1224631
  • Camos, V., Lagner, P., & Barrouillet, P. (2009). Two maintenance mechanisms of verbal information in working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(3), 457–469. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.06.002
  • Camos, V., Mora, G., & Barrouillet, P. (2013). Phonological similarity effect in complex span task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1927–1950. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.768275
  • Camos, V., Mora, G., & Oberauer, K. (2011). Adaptive choice between articulatory rehearsal and attentional refreshing in verbal working memory. Memory & Cognition, 39, 231–244. doi: 10.3758/s13421-010-0011-x
  • Cattell, R. B., & Cattell, A. K. S. (1973). Culture fair test of “g”. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.
  • Chow, M., Macnamara, B. N., & Conway, A. R. (2016). Phonological similarity in working memory span tasks. Memory & Cognition, 44, 937–949. doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0609-8
  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 33, 497–505. doi: 10.1080/14640748108400805
  • Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 769–786. doi: 10.3758/BF03196772
  • Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 547–552. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.005
  • Cortis, C., Dent, K., Kennett, S., & Ward, G. (2015). First things first: Similar list length and output order effects for verbal and nonverbal stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1179–1214. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000086
  • Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. New York, NY: Oxford Univsersity Press.
  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–114. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01003922
  • Cowan, N., Keller, T. A., Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., McDougall, S., & Rack, J. (1994). Verbal memory span in children: Speech Timing Clues to the mechanisms Underlying Age and word length effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 234–250.
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
  • Félix, S. B., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Nairne, J. S. (2019). Adaptive memory: Longevity and learning intentionality of the animacy effect. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 31, 251–260. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2019.1586716
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124.
  • Gelin, M., Bonin, P., Meot, A., & Bugaiska, A. (2018). Do animacy effects persist in memory for context? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 965–974. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1307866
  • Gelin, M., Bugaiska, A., Meot, A., & Bonin, P. (2017). Are animacy effects in episodic memory independent of encoding instructions? Memory (Hove, England), 25, 2–18. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2015.1117643
  • Gelin, M., Bugaiska, A., Meot, A., Vinter, A., & Bonin, P. (2019). Animacy effects in episodic memory: Do imagery processes really play a role? Memory (Hove, England), 27, 209–223. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1498108
  • Giofre, D., & Mammarella, I. C. (2014). The relationship between working memory and intelligence in children: Is the scoring procedure important? Intelligence, 46, 300–310. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.08.001
  • Grenfell-Essam, R., & Ward, G. (2012). Examining the relationship between free recall and immediate serial recall: The role of list length, strategy use, and test expectancy. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 106–148. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.04.004
  • Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In J. F. Delafresnaye (Ed.), Brain mechanisms and learning (pp. 37–46). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D. A., Martin, S., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1217–1232.
  • Hulme, C., Stuart, G., Brown, G. D. A., & Morin, C. (2003). High- and low-frequency words are recalled equally well in alternating lists: Evidence for associative effects in serial recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 500–518. doi: 10.1016/s0749-596x(03)00096-2
  • Hulme, C., Suprenant, A. M., Bireta, T. J., Stuart, G., & Neath, I. (2004). Abolishing the word-length effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 98–106. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.98
  • Hurlstone, M. J., Hitch, G. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2014). Memory for serial order across domains: An overview of the literature and directions for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 339–373. doi: 10.1037/a0034221
  • Jones, G., & Macken, B. (2015). Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition, 144, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009
  • Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189
  • Kanerva, K., & Kalakoski, V. (2016). The predictive utility of a working memory span task depends on processing demand and the cognitive task. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30, 681–690. doi: 10.1002/acp.3243
  • Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Effects of emotion on memory specificity in young and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, P208–P215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x
  • Klein, S. B., Robertson, T. E., & Delton, A. W. (2010). Facing the future: Memory as an evolved system for planning future acts. Memory & Cognition, 38, 13–22. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.1.13
  • Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). An introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284. doi: 10.1080/01638539809545028
  • Leding, J. K. (2019a). Adaptive memory: Animacy, threat, and attention in free recall. Memory & Cognition, 47, 383–394. doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0873-x
  • Leding, J. K. (2019b). Intentional memory and online data collection: A test of the effects of animacy and threat on episodic memory. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 31, 4–15. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2018.1564756
  • Meinhardt, M. J., Bell, R., Buchner, A., & Roer, J. P. (2018). Adaptive memory: Is the animacy effect on memory due to emotional arousal? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 1399–1404. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1485-y
  • Mieth, L., Roer, J. P., Buchner, A., & Bell, R. (2019). Adaptive memory: Enhanced source memory for animate entities. Memory (Hove, England), 27, 1034–1042. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2019.1617882
  • Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 50, 528–559. doi: 10.1080/027249897392017
  • Murphy, K., Roodenrys, S., & Fox, A. (2006). Event-related potential correlates of the word length effect in working memory. Brain Research, 1112, 179–190. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.07.007
  • Nairne, J. S. (1990). A feature model of immediate memory. Memory & Cognition, 18, 251–269. doi: 10.3758/BF03213879
  • Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2008). Adaptive memory: Remembering with a stone-age brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 239–243. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00582.x
  • Nairne, J. S., VanArsdall, J. E., & Cogdill, M. (2017). Remembering the living: Epidsodic memory is tuned to animacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 22–27. doi:10.1177/0963721416667711.
  • Nairne, J. S., VanArsdall, J. E., Pandeirada, J. N. S., Cogdill, M., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Adaptive memory: The mnemonic value of animacy. Psychological Science, 24, 2099–2105. doi: 10.1177/0956797613480803
  • Oberauer, K. (2005). Binding and inhibition in working memory: Individual and age differences in short-term recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 368–387. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.368
  • Oberauer, K. (2009). Design for a working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 45–100. doi: 10.1016/s0079-7421(09)51002-x
  • Oberauer, K., Farrell, S., Jarrold, C., & Lewandowsky, S. (2016). What limits working memory capacity? Psychological Bulletin, 142, 758–799. doi: 10.1037/bul0000046
  • Oberauer, K., Lewandowsky, S., Awh, E., Brown, G. D. A., Conway, A., Cowan, N., … Ward, G. (2018). Benchmarks for models of short-term and working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 885–958. doi: 10.1037/bul0000153
  • Oberauer, K., Süß, H.-M., Wilhelm, O., & Sander, N. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and reasoning ability. In A. R. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 49–75). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Opfer, J. E., & Gelman, S. A. (2011). Development of the animate–inanimate distinction. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 213–238). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Popp, E. Y., & Serra, M. J. (2016). Adaptive memory: Animacy enhances free recall but impairs cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 186–201. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000174
  • Popp, E. Y., & Serra, M. J. (2018). The animacy advantage for free-recall performance is not attributable to greater mental arousal. Memory (Hove, England), 26, 89–95. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1326507
  • Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate-inaniamte distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–228. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.209
  • Raye, C. L., Johnson, M. K., Mitchell, K. J., Greene, E. J., & Johnson, M. R. (2007). Refreshing: A minimal executive function. Cortex, 43, 135–145. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70451-9
  • Roodenrys, S., Hulme, C., Lethbridge, A., Hinton, M., & Nimmo, L. M. (2002). Word-frequency and phonological-neighborhood effects on verbal short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 1019–1034. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1019
  • Roodenrys, S., & Quinlan, P. T. (2000). The effects of stimulus set size and word frequency on verbal serial recall. Memory (Hove, England), 8, 71–78. doi: 10.1080/096582100387623
  • Rowell, S. F., Green, J. S., Teachman, B. A., & Salthouse, T. A. (2016). Age does not matter: Memory complaints are related to negative affect throughout adulthood. Aging & Mental Health, 20, 1255–1263. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1078284
  • Salame, P., & Baddeley, A. D. (1982). Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech: Implications for the structure of working memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 150–164. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90521-7
  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science’s Compass, 283, 1657–1661.
  • Soares, S. C., Esteves, F., Lundqvist, D., & Ohman, A. (2009). Some animal specific fears are more specific than others: Evidence from attention and emotion measures. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 1032–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.022
  • Soares, S. C., Lindstrom, B., Esteves, F., & Ohman, A. (2014). The hidden snake in the grass: Superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions. PLos ONE, 9(12), 1–26. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114724
  • Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Davis, C. J. (2006). The Bristol norms for age of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 598–605. doi: 10.3758/BF03193891
  • St Clair-Thompson, H. (2012). Ascending versus randomised list lengths in working memory span tasks. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 335–341. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.639760
  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–229). New York, NY: Oxford Univeristy Press.
  • VanArsdall, J. E., Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Blunt, J. R. (2013). Adaptive memory: Animacy processing produces mnemonic advantages. Experimental Psychology, 60, 172–178. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000186
  • VanArsdall, J. E., Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Cogdill, M. (2017). A categorical recall strategy does not explain animacy effects in episodic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 761–771. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1159707
  • Van Overschelde, J. P., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the Battig and Montague (1969) norms. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 289–335. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.10.003
  • Warriner, A. B., Kuperman, V., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1191–1207. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
  • Xiao, X., Dong, Q., Chen, C., & Xue, G. (2016). Neural pattern similarity underlies the mnemonic advantages for living words. Cortex, 79, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.03.016
  • Yang, H., Yang, S., & Isen, A. M. (2013). Positive affect improves working memory: Implications for controlled cognitive processing. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 474–482. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.713325

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.