306
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Exploring the contribution of phonological memory to metasyntactic abilities in bilingual children

&
Pages 78-95 | Received 10 Nov 2015, Accepted 14 Jun 2017, Published online: 04 Jul 2017

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2002). Reading, syntactic, orthographic, and working memory skills of bilingual Arab Canadian children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 661–678.
  • Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: Implications for language development. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35, 95–116.
  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A.-M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85–106.
  • Andrade, J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2011). The contribution of phonological short-term memory to artificial grammar learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 960–974.
  • Ayoun, D. (1999). The verb movement parameter in French L2 acquisition. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 2, 103–125.
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417–423.
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psychologist, 7, 85–97.
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.
  • Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advance in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory: The multiple-component model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 28–61). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 169–181.
  • Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325–339.
  • Bourque Richard, J. (1998). Les distorsions mnémoniques en fonction de l’âge, le vocabulaire et la suggestibilité [Memory distortions as a function of age, vocabulary, and suggestability] (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Université de Moncton, Moncton.
  • Bowey, J. A. (1986a). Syntactic awareness and verbal performance from preschool to fifth grade. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 285–308.
  • Bowey, J. A. (1986b). Syntactic awareness in relation to reading skill and ongoing reading comprehension monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 282–299.
  • Bowey, J. A. (2005). Grammatical sensitivity: Its origins and potential contribution to early word reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 318–343.
  • Brimo, D., Apel, K., & Fountain, T. (2017). Examining the contributions of syntactic awareness and syntactic knowledge to reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 40, 57–74.
  • Cain, K. (2007). Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there evidence for a special relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 679–694.
  • Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867–873.
  • Chiappe, P., & Siegel, L. S. (1999). Phonological awareness and reading acquisition in English and Punjabi-speaking Canadian children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 20–28.
  • Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (2002). Reading-related skills of kindergartners from diverse linguistic backgrounds. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 95–116.
  • Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2002). Linguistic diversity and the development of reading skills: A longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 369–400.
  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Da Fontoura, H., & Siegel, L. S. (1995). Reading, syntactic, and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese-English Canadian children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 139–153.
  • Davidson, D., Raschke, V. R., & Pervez, J. (2010). Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu-English) children. Cognitive Development, 25, 166–182.
  • Demont, E. (1994). Développement métalinguistique et apprentissage de la lecture [Metalinguistic development and learning to read] (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Université de Bourgogne, Dijon.
  • Dunn, L. M., Theriault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody [French Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test]. Belgique: ATM.
  • Edwards, J., Beckman, M. E., & Munson, B. (2004). The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 421–436.
  • Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 141–172.
  • Erlam, R. (2006). Elicited imitation as a measure of L2 implicit knowledge: An empirical validation study. Applied Linguistics, 27, 464–491.
  • Fortier, V., Simard, D., & French, L. M. (2012). L'utilisation d'une mesure de la mémoire phonologique auprès d'une population d'enfants linguistiquement hétérogène [Measuring phonological memory with a linguistically heterogeneous children population]. The Canadian Modern Language Review/La Revue canadienne des langues vivantes, 68, 291–315.
  • French, L. M. (2006). Phonological working memory and second language acquisition. A developmental study of francophone children learning English in Quebec. New York, NY: The Edwin Mellen Press.
  • French, L. M., & O'Brien, I. (2008). Phonological memory and children's second language grammar learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 463–487.
  • Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513–543.
  • Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H. (1994). The children's test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2, 103–127.
  • Gaux, C., & Gombert, J.-E. (1999). Implicit and explicit syntactic knowledge and reading in pre-adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 169–188.
  • Gombert, J.-E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Gombert, J.-E. (1993). Metacognition, metalanguage and metapragmatics. International Journal of Psychology, 28, 571–580.
  • Gottardo, A., Collins, P., Baciu, I., & Gebotys, R. (2008). Predictors of Grade 2 word reading and vocabulary learning from Grade 1 variables in Spanish-speaking children: Similarities and differences. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 23, 11–24.
  • Hanten, G., & Martin, R. C. (2000). Contributions of phonological and semantic short-term memory to sentence processing: Evidence from two cases of closed head injury in children. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 335–361.
  • Lefrançois, P., & Armand, F. (2003). The role of phonological and syntactic awareness in second-language reading: The case of Spanish-speaking learners of French. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 219–246.
  • Lesaux, N. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). The development of reading in children who speak English as a second language. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1005–1119.
  • Lust, B., Flynn, S., & Foley, C. (1996). What children know about what they say: Elicited imitation as a research method for assessing children's syntax. In D. McDaniel, C. McKee, & H. Smith Cairns (Eds.), Methods for assessing children's syntax (pp. 55–76). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Martin, K. I., & Ellis, N. C. (2012). The roles of phonological short-term memory and working memory in L2 grammar and vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 379–413.
  • McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1987a). Understanding: A function of short-term memory? Brain, 110, 1565–1578.
  • McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1987b). The double dissociation of short-term memory for lists and sentences. Brain, 110, 1545–1563.
  • Montrul, S. (1998). The L2 acquisition of dative experiencer subjects. Second Language Research, 14, 27–61.
  • Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443–468.
  • O'Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Collentine, J., & Freed, B. (2006). Phonological memory and lexical, narrative, and grammatical skills in second language oral production by adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 377–402.
  • O'Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Freed, B., & Collentine, J. (2007). Phonological memory predicts second language oral fluency gains. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 557–582.
  • Papagno, C., Cecchetto, C., Reati, F., & Bello, L. (2007). Processing of syntactically complex sentences relies on verbal short-term memory: Evidence from a short-term memory patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 292–311.
  • Plaza, M. (2001). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic awareness, and reading: A longitudinal study from kindergarten to Grade 1. First Language, 21, 3–24.
  • Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2003). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic awareness, and naming speed in the reading and spelling performance of first-grade children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 287–292.
  • Poncelet, M., & Van der Linden, M. (2003). L’évaluation du stock phonologique de la mémoire de travail: Élaboration d'une épreuve de répétition de non-mots pour population francophone [Evaluating the phonological storage capacity of working memory: Creation of a nonword repetition task for a French speaking population]. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 13, 377–407.
  • Potocki, A., Ecalle, J., & Magnan, A. (2013). Narrative comprehension skills in 5-year-old children: Correlational analysis and comprehender profiles. The Journal of Educational Research, 106, 14–26.
  • Rego, L. L. B., & Bryant, P. E. (1993). The connection between phonological, syntactic and semantic skills and children's reading and spelling. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 235–246.
  • Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 155–172.
  • Siegel, L. S. (1993). The development of reading. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 63–97). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Simard, D., & Fortier, V. (2008). Metasyntactic ability development among submersion and French native elementary school children: Investigation of task demand. In Z. Han (Ed.), Understanding second language process (pp. 160–175). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
  • Simard, D., Fortier, V., & Foucambert, D. (2013). Measuring metasyntactic ability among heritage language children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 19–31.
  • Simard, D., Foucambert, D., & Labelle, M. (2013). An examination of the contribution of syntactic and metasyntactic abilities in reading comprehension in L1 and L2. In K. Roehr & G. A. Gánem-Gutiérrez (Eds.), The metalinguistic dimension in instructed L2 learning (pp. 45–69). London, UK: Continuum.
  • Simard, D., Foucambert, D., & Labelle, M. (2014). Examining the contribution of metasyntactic ability to reading comprehension among native and non-native speakers of French. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18, 586–604.
  • Simard, D., Labelle, M., & Bergeron, A. (2017). Measuring metasyntactic abilities: On a classification of metasyntactic tasks. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 433–456. doi:10.1007/s10936-016-9445-z
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
  • Verhagen, J., Leseman, P., & Messer, M. (2015). Phonological memory and the acquisition of grammar in child L2 learners. Language Learning, 65, 417–448.
  • Vinther, T. (2002). Elicited imitation: A brief overview. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 12, 54–73.
  • Weinberg, S., & Abramowitz, S. (2002). Data analysis for the behavioral sciences using SPSS. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Williams, J. N., & Lovatt, P. (2003). Phonological memory and rule learning. Language Learning, 53, 67–121.
  • Willis, C. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Phonological short-term memory contribution to sentence processing in young children. Memory, 9, 349–363.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.