2,015
Views
31
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ARTICLES

Mind Wandering and the Incubation Effect in Insight Problem Solving

, , &

REFERENCES

  • Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Evidence for the default network's role in spontaneous cognition. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104, 322–335. doi:10.1152/jn.00830.2009
  • Baars, B. J. (2010). Spontaneous repetitive thoughts can be adaptive: Postscript on “mind wandering”. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 208–210. doi:10.1037/a0018726
  • Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806. doi:10.1037/a0012815
  • Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2011). When prevention promotes creativity: The role of mood, regulatory focus, and regulatory closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 794–809. doi:10.1037/a0022981
  • Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23, 1117–1122. doi:10.1177/0956797612446024
  • Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, P. S., Bellgowan, S. F., Rao, S. M., & Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: A functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences, 11, 80–93. doi:10.1162/089892999563265
  • Chein, J. M., Weisberg, R. W., Streeter, N. L., & Kwok, S. (2010). Working memory and insight in the nine-dot problem. Memory & Cognition, 38, 883–892. doi:10.3758/mc.38.7.883
  • Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 8719–8724. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900234106
  • Cinan, S., & Dogan, A. (2013). Working memory, mental prospection, time orientation, and cognitive Insight. Journal of Individual Differences, 34, 159–169. doi:10.1027/1614–0001/a000111
  • Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (2002). Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning and consciousness (pp. 1–40). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
  • De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 656–669. doi:10.1177/0146167211435795
  • Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79, 1–37. doi:10.1016/s0010–0277(00)00123–2
  • Fleck, J. I. (2008). Working memory demands in insight versus analytic problem solving. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20, 139–176. doi:10.1080/09541440601016954
  • Haider, H., & Frensch, P. (2009). Conflicts between expected and actually performed behavior lead to verbal report of incidentally acquired sequential knowledge. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 73, 817–834.
  • Horvitz, L. A. (2002) Eureka!: Scientific breakthroughs that changed the world. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Klinger, E. (2009). Daydreaming and fantasizing: Thought flow and motivation. In K. D. Markman W. M. P. Klein & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 225–239). New York, NY: Psychology Press
  • Klinger, E. (2013). Goal commitments and the content of thoughts and dreams: Basic principles. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 415. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00415
  • Lavric, A., Forstmeier, S., & Rippon, G. (2000). Differences in working memory involvement in analytical and creative tasks: An ERP study. NeuroReport, 11, 1613–1618. doi:10.1097/00001756–200006050-00004
  • Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393–395. doi:10.1126/science.1131295
  • McMillan, R. L., Kaufman, S. B., & Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 626. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00626
  • Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). ‘Oops!’: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35, 747–758. doi:10.1016/S0028–3932(97)00015–8
  • Sakaki, M., & Niki, K. (2011). Effects of the brief viewing of emotional stimuli on understanding of insight solutions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 526–540. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0051-0
  • Schooler, J. W. (2002). Re-representing consciousness: Dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 339–344. doi:10.1016/s1364–6613(02)01949–6
  • Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (2005). Zoning-out during reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children (pp. 204–226). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  • Singer, J. L. (1974). Daydreaming and the stream of thought: Daydreams have usually been associated with idleness and inattentiveness. Now, however, through an empirical research program, their general function and adaptive possibilities are being elucidated. American Scientist, 62, 417–425.
  • Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in daydreaming and related inner experience. American Psychologist, 30, 727–738. doi:10.1037/h0076928
  • Singer, J. L., & Antrobus, J. S. (1972). Daydreaming, imaginal processes, and personality: A normative study. In P. W. Sheehan (Ed.), The function and nature of imagery (pp. 175–202). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Sio, U., & Ormerod, T. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 94–120.
  • Smallwood, J., Beach, E., Schooler, J. W., & Handy, T. C. (2008). Going AWOL in the brain: Mind wandering reduces cortical analysis of external events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 458–469. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20.3.458
  • Smallwood, J., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., O'Connor, R., & Obonsawin, M. (2004). Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 657–690. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2004.06.003
  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946–958. doi:10.1037/0033–2909.132.6.946
  • Smeekens, B. A. (2013). The role of working memory capacity and mind wandering in creativity and insight (Master's thesis). The University of North Carolina at Greensboro.
  • Smith, C. (2006). A role of REM sleep in offline memory reprocessing. Brain and Cognition, 60, 331–331.
  • Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maquet, P., & D'Argembeau, A. (2011). Neural correlates of ongoing conscious experience: Both task-unrelatedness and stimulus-independence are related to default network activity. PLoS ONE, 6, e16997. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016997
  • Sternberg, R. J., & Davidson, J. E. (Eds.). (1995). The nature of insight. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Subramaniam, K., Kounios, J., Parrish, T. B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2009). A brain mechanism for facilitation of insight by positive affect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 415–432. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21057
  • Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. R., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 498–505. doi:10.3758/bf03192720
  • Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight. Nature, 427, 352–355. doi:10.1038/nature02223
  • Wang, K., Yu, C., Xu, L., Qin, W., Li, K., Xu, L., & Jiang, T. (2009). Offline memory reprocessing: Involvement of the brain's default network in spontaneous thought processes. PLoS ONE, 4, e4867. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004867
  • White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Uninhibited imaginations: Creativity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1121–1131. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.007
  • White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2011). Creative style and achievement in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 673–677. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.015
  • Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. F. (2012a). How working memory capacity affects problem solving. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 56, 185–227. doi:10.1016/b978–0-12–394393-4.00006–6
  • Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. F. (2012b). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 258–262. doi:10.1177/0963721412447622
  • Williams, F. E. (1979). Assessing creativity across williams cube model. Gifted Child Quarterly, 23, 748–756.
  • Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Verleger, R., Bataghva, Z., Born, J., & Wagner, U. (2008). Shifting from implicit to explicit knowledge: Different roles of early- and late-night sleep. Learning & Memory, 15, 508–515.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.