3,117
Views
35
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Articulating What Infants Attune to in Native Speech

, , &

References

  • Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46, 155–182.
  • Aslin, R. N., & Pisoni, D. B. (1980). Some developmental processes in speech perception. In G. H. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology (Vol. 2, pp. 67–96). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Aslin, R. N., Saffran, J. R., & Newport, E. L. (1998). Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 9, 321–324.
  • Auban, J., & Ménard, L. (2006). Compensation for a labial perturbation: An acoustic and articulatory study of child and adult French speakers. Proceedings of International Symposium on Speech Production (ISSP). Ubatuba, Brazil: CEFALA.
  • Aziz-Zadeh, L., Iacoboni, M., Zaidel, E., Wilson, S., & Mazziotta, J. (2004). Left hemisphere motor facilitation in response to manual action sounds. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 2609–2612.
  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The Celex lexical database. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania. Linguistic Data Consortium.
  • Best, C. T. (1993). Emergence of language-specific constraints in perception of non-native speech contrasts: A window on early phonological development. In B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. MacNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life (pp. 289–304). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.
  • Best, C. T. (1994). Learning to perceive the sound pattern of English. In C. Rovee-Collier, & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 9, pp. 217–304). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  • Best, C. T. (1995). A direct realist perspective on cross-language speech perception. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and methodological issues in cross-language speech research (pp. 167–200). Timonium, MD: York Press.
  • Best, C. T. (in press). Perceiving non-native click consonant contrasts. In B. Sands (Ed.), The handbook of click languages. Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill.
  • Best, C. T., Kroos, C. H., & Irwin, J. (2010). Now I see what you said: Infant sensitivity to place congruency between audio-only and silent-video presentations of native and nonnative consonants. Proceedings of AVSP (AudioVisual Speech Perception). Hakone, Japan: AVISA.
  • Best, C. T., Kroos, C. H., & Irwin, J. (2011). Do infants detect A→V articulator congruency for nonnative click consonants? Proceedings of AVSP (AudioVisual Speech Perception). Volterra, Italy: AVISA.
  • Best, C. T., Kroos, C. H., & Irwin, J. (2014, July). Baby steps in perceiving articulatory foundations of phonological contrasts: Infants detect audio→video congruency in native and nonnative consonants. Paper presented at Laboratory Phonology, Tokyo, Japan.
  • Best, C. T., & McRoberts, G. W. (2003). Infant perception of nonnative consonant contrasts that adults assimilate in different ways. Language & Speech, 46, 183–216.
  • Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Goodell, E. (2001). American listeners' perception of nonnative consonant contrasts varying in perceptual assimilation to English phonology. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1097, 775–794.
  • Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., LaFleur, R., & Silver-Isenstadt, J. (1995). Divergent developmental patterns for infants' perception of two non-native consonant contrasts. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 339–350.
  • Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Sithole, N. M. (1988). Examination of perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by English-speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 45–60.
  • Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In M. Munro, & O.-S. Bohn (Eds.), Second language speech learning (pp. 13–34). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
  • Blake, J., & de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1992). Patterns in babbling: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 19, 51–74.
  • Bosch, L., & Ramon-Casas, M. (2011). Variability in vowel production by bilingual speakers: Can input properties hinder the early stabilization of contrastive categories? Journal of Phonetics, 39, 514–526.
  • Bradlow, A. R., Pisoni, D. B., Akahane-Yamada, R., & Tohkura, Y. I. (1997). Training Japanese listeners to identify English/r/and/l: IV: Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 2299–2310.
  • Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological units. Phonology, 6, 201–251.
  • Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1992). Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica, 49, 155–180.
  • Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (2000). Competing constraints on intergestural coordination and self-organization of phonological structures. Les Cahiers de l'ICP: Bulletin de la Communication Parlée, 5, 25–34.
  • Brown, R. (1958). Words and things. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
  • Bruderer, A. G., Danielson, D. K., Kandhadai, P., & Werker, J. F. (2015). Sensorimotor influences on speech perception in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 13531–13536.
  • Burnham, D., & Dodd, B. (2004). Auditory–visual speech integration by prelinguistic infants: Perception of an emergent consonant in the McGurk effect. Developmental Psychobiology, 45, 204–220.
  • Chen, L. M., & Kent, R. (2010). Segmental production in Mandarin-learning infants. Journal of Child Language, 37, 341–371.
  • Chen, X., Striano, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2004). Auditory–oral matching behavior in newborns. Developmental Science, 7, 42–47.
  • Cruttenden, A. (1970). A phonetic study of babbling. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 5, 110–117.
  • Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, C., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 492–504.
  • Cutler, A. (2008). The abstract representations in speech processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1601–1619.
  • Davis, B. L., & MacNeilage, P. F. (1995). The articulatory basis of babbling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1199–211.
  • d'Ausilio, A., Bufalari, I., Salmas, P., & Fadiga, L. (2009). The motor somatotopy of speech perception. Current Biology, 19, 381–385.
  • de Boysson-Bardies, B., Sagart, L., & Bacri, N. (1981). Phonetic analysis of late babbling: A case study of a French child. Journal of Child Language, 8, 511–524.
  • de Boysson-Bardies, B., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1984). Discernible differences in the babbling of infants according to target language. Journal of Child Language, 11, 1–15.
  • de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language, 67, 297–319.
  • de Boysson-Bardies, B., Vihman, M. M., Roug-Hellichius, L., Durand, C., Landberg, I., & Arao, F. (1992). Material evidence of infant selection from the target language: A cross-linguistic phonetic study. In C. A. Ferguson, L. Menn, & C. Stoel-Gammon (Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (pp. 369–391). Timonium, MD: York Press.
  • DePaolis, R. A., Vihman, M. M., & Keren-Portnoy, T. (2011). Do production patterns influence the processing of speech in prelinguistic infants? Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 590–601.
  • DePaolis, R. A., Vihman, M. M., & Nakai, S. (2013). The influence of babbling patterns on the processing of speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 642–649.
  • Derrick, D., & Gick, B. (2013). Aerotactile integration from distal skin stimuli. Multisensory Research, 26, 405–416.
  • Desjardins, R. N., & Werker, J. F. (2004). Is the integration of heard and seen speech mandatory for infants? Developmental Psychobiology, 45, 187–203.
  • Eilers, R. E. (1977). Context-sensitive perception of naturally produced stop and fricative consonants by infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 61, 1321–1336.
  • Eilers, R. E., Gavin, W., & Wilson, W. R. (1979). Linguistic experience and phonemic perception in infancy: A cross-linguistic study. Child Development, 50, 14–18.
  • Eilers, R. E., & Minifie, F. D. (1975). Fricative discrimination in early infancy. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 18, 158–167.
  • Eilers, R. E., Wilson, W. R., & Moore, J. M. (1977). Developmental changes in speech discrimination in infants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 20, 766–780.
  • Eimas, P. D., & Miller, J. L. (1980). Discrimination of information for manner of articulation. Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 367–375.
  • Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.
  • Engstrand, O., Williams, K., & Lacerda, F. (2003). Does babbling sound native? Listener responses to vocalizations produced by Swedish and American 12- and 18-month-olds. Phonetica, 60, 17–44.
  • Esling, J. H. (1996). Pharyngeal consonants and the aryepiglottic sphincter. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 26, 65–88.
  • Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., & Olivier, E. (2005). Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others' action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 213–218.
  • Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2608–2611.
  • Fagan, M. K., & Iverson, J. M. (2007). The influence of mouthing on infant vocalization. Infancy, 11, 191–202.
  • Fenwick, S. E., Davis, C., Best, C. T. & Tyler, M. (2015). The effect of modality and speaking style on the discrimination of non-native phonological and phonetic contrasts in noise. FAAVSP-2015, 67–72.
  • Flege, J. E. (1984). The effect of linguistic experience on Arabs' perception of the English /s/ vs. /z/ contrast. Folia Linguistica, 18, 117–138.
  • Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. R. A. (2004). Perceiving vowels in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 1–34.
  • Fowler, C. A. (2004). Speech as a supramodal or amodal phenomenon. In G. A. Calvert, C. Spence, & B. E. Stein (Eds.), The handbook of multisensory processes (pp. 189–201). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Fowler, C. A., & Dekle, D. (1991). Listening with eye and hand: Cross-modal contributions to speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 816–828.
  • Fowler, C. A., & Galantucci, B. (2008). The relation of speech perception and speech production. In D. B. Pisoni, & R. Remez (Eds.), The handbook of speech perception (pp. 632–652). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Fowler, C. A., Rubin, P., Remez, R., & Turvey, M. (1980). Implications for speech production of a general theory of action. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production, 1: Speech and talk (pp. 373–420). London, UK: Academic Press.
  • Friederici, A. D., & Wessels, J. M. (1993). Phonotactic knowledge of word boundaries and its use in infant speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 287–295.
  • Geddes, D. T., Kent, J. C., Mitoulas, L. R., & Hartmann, P. E. (2008). Tongue movement and intra-oral vacuum in breastfeeding infants. Early Human Development, 84, 471–477.
  • Ghosh, P. K., Goldstein, L. M., & Narayanan, S. S. (2011). Processing speech signals using auditory-like filterbank provides least uncertainty about articulatory gestures. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129, 4014–4022.
  • Gick, B., & Derrick, D. (2009). Aero-tactile integration in speech perception. Nature, 462, 502–504.
  • Gick, B., Ikegami, Y., & Derrick, D. (2010). The temporal window of audio-tactile integration in speech perception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, EL342–EL346.
  • Goldstein, L. M. (2003). Emergence of discrete gestures. Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 15, 85–88.
  • Goldstein, L. M., Byrd, D., & Saltzman, E. (2006). The role of vocal tract gestural action units in understanding the evolution of phonology. In M. A. Arbib (Ed.), Action to language via the mirror neuron system (pp. 215–249). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Goldstein, L. M., & Fowler, C. A. (2003). Articulatory phonology: A phonology for public language use. In N. O. Schiller, & A. S. Meyer (Eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production: Differences and similarities (pp. 159–207). Berlin, Germany: de Gruyter.
  • Goldstein, L. M., Nam, H., Kulthreshtha, M., Root, L., & Best, C. (2008, June). Distribution of tongue tip articulations in Hindi versus English and the acquisition of stop place categories. Paper presented at Laboratory Phonology 11, Wellington, New Zealand.
  • Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 8030–8035.
  • Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants' babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19, 515–523.
  • Gottlieb, G. (1976). The roles of experience in the development of behavior and the nervous system. In G. Gottlieb (Ed.), Development of neural and behavioral specificity (pp. 1–35). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Gottlieb, G. (1981). The roles of experience in species-specific perceptual development. In R. N. Aslin, J. R. Alberts, & M. R. Peterson (Eds.), Development of perception: Psychobiological perspectives: Vol. 1. Audition, somatic perception and the chemical senses (pp. 5–44). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., Goldstein, M. H., & King, A. P. (2006). Mothers provide differential feedback to infants' prelinguistic sounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 509–516.
  • Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., & King, A. P. (2016). The influence of interactive context on prelinguistic vocalizations and maternal responses. Language Learning & Development, 12, 280–294.
  • Guion, S., Flege, J. E., Akahane-Yamada, R., & Pruitt, J. C. (2000). An investigation of current models of second language speech perception: The case of Japanese adults' perception of English consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 107, 2711–2724.
  • Hayashi, Y., Hoashi, E., & Nara, T. (1997). Ultrasonographic analysis of sucking behavior of newborn infants: The driving force of sucking pressure. Early Human Development, 49(1), 33–38.
  • Holmberg, T. L., Morgan, K. A., & Kuhl, P. K. (1977). Speech perception in early infancy: Discrimination of fricative consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 62, S99.
  • Irwin, O. C. (1947). Development of speech during infancy: Curve of phonemic frequencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 187–193.
  • Irwin, O. C. (1948). Infant speech: Development of vowel sounds. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 13, 31–34.
  • Iwayama, K., & Eishima, M. (1997). Neonatal sucking behaviour and its development until 14 months. Early Human Development, 47(1), 1–9.
  • Ito, T., Tiede, M., & Ostry, D. J. (2009). Somatosensory function in speech perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106, 1245–1248.
  • Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton. ( English translation of R. Jakobson, 1941, Kindersprache, aphasie und allgemeine lautgesetze. Uppsala, Sweden: Almqvist & Wiksell).
  • Jusczyk, P. W. (1997). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J., Svenkerud, V. Y., & Jusczyk, A. M. (1993). Infants' sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 402–420.
  • Jusczyk, P. W., & Luce, P. A. (1994). Infants′ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630–645.
  • Kelso, J. A. S., Tuller, B., Vatikiotis-Bateson, E., & Fowler, C. A. (1984). Functionally specific articulatory cooperation following jaw perturbations during speech: Evidence for coordinative structures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 812–832.
  • Kencalo, T. A., Best, C. T., Tyler, M. D., & Goldstein, L. M. (2007). Perception of native and non-native fricative contrasts by Ukrainian-Australian English bilinguals and Australian English monolinguals. Australian Journal of Psychology, 59, 40.
  • Kent, R. D., & Murray, A. D. (1982). Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 72, 353–365.
  • Keren-Portnoy, T., Majorano, M., & Vihman, M. M. (2009). From phonetics to phonology: The emergence of first words in Italian. Journal of Child Language, 36, 235–267.
  • Kern, S., Davis, B. L., & Zink, I. (2009). From babbling to first words in four languages: Common trends, cross language and individual differences. In F. d’Errico & J.-M. Hombert (Eds.), Becoming eloquent: Advances in the emergence of language, human cognition and modern culture (pp. 205–232). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
  • Kohler, E., Keysers, C. M., Umilta, A., Fogassi, L., Galles, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848.
  • Kokkinaki, T., & Kugiumutzakis, G. (2000). Basic aspects of vocal imitation in infant-parent interaction during the first 6 months. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 18, 173–187.
  • Konopczinski, G. (1990). Le langage émergent: Caractéristiques rythmiques [Emerging language: Rhythmic characteristics]. Hamburg, Germany: Buske Verlag.
  • Kuhl, P. K. (1993). Innate predispositions and the effects of experience in speech perception: The native language magnet theory. In B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. MacNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life (pp. 259–274). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.
  • Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 5, 831–843.
  • Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T, Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 979–1000.
  • Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T., & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception and later language development: Implications for the “critical period.” Language Learning and Development, 1, 237–264.
  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1141.
  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1984). The intermodal representation of speech in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 361–381.
  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 2425–2438.
  • Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9, F13–F21.
  • Kushnerenko, E., Teinonen, T., Volein, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Electrophysiological evidence of illusory audiovisual speech percept in human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 11442–11445.
  • Lasky, R. E., Syrdal-Lasky, A., & Klein, R. E. (1975). VOT discrimination by four to six and a half month old infants from Spanish environments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 215–225.
  • Lee, S. A. S., Davis, B., & MacNeilage, P. (2010). Universal production patterns and ambient language influences in babbling: A cross-linguistic study of Korean- and English-learning infants. Journal of Child Language, 37, 293–318.
  • Lee-Kim, S. I., Kawahara, S., & Lee, S. J. (2014). The ‘whistled’ fricative in Xitsonga: Its articulation and acoustics. Phonetica, 71, 50–81.
  • Levitt, A. G., & Aydelott Utman, J. G. (1992). From babbling towards the sound systems of English and French: A longitudinal case study. Journal of Child Language, 19, 19–49.
  • Levitt, A., Jusczyk, P. W., Murray, J., & Carden, G. (1988). Context effects in two-month-old infants' perception of labiodental/interdental fricative contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 361.
  • Levitt, A. G., & Wang, Q. (1991). Evidence for language-specific rhythmic influences in the reduplicative babbling of French- and English-learning infants. Language and Speech, 34, 235–249.
  • Liberman, A. M. (1996). Speech: A special code. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
  • Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461.
  • Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. (1985). The motor theory revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36.
  • Lotto, A. J., Hickok, G. S., & Holt, L. L. (2009). Reflections on mirror neurons and speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 110–114.
  • MacKain, K. (1982). On assessing the role of experience on infants' speech discrimination. Journal of Child Language, 9, 527–542.
  • MacKain, K., Best, C. T., & Strange, W. (1981). Categorical perception of /r/ and /l/ by Japanese bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 369–390.
  • MacKain, K., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., & Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. Science, 219, 1347–1349.
  • MacNeilage,, P. F., Davis, B. L., Kinney, A., & Matyear, C. L. (2000). The motor core of speech: A comparison of serial organization patterns in infants and languages. Child Development, 71, 153–163.
  • Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns' cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology, 19, 1994–1997.
  • Marslen-Wilson, W., & Zwitserlood, P. (1989). Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 576.
  • Massaro, D. W. (1984). Children's perception of visual and auditory speech. Child Development, 55, 1777–1788.
  • Matyear, C. L., MacNeilage, P. F., & Davis, B. L. (1998). Nasalization of vowels in nasal environments in babbling: evidence for frame dominance. Phonetica, 55, 1–17.
  • Maye, J., Weiss, D. J., & Aslin, R. N. (2008). Statistical phonetic learning in infants: Facilitation and feature generalization. Developmental Science, 11, 122–134.
  • Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101–B111.
  • McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.
  • Mehler, J., Peña, M., Nespor, M., & Bonatti, L. (2006). The “soul” of language does not use statistics: Reflections on vowels and consonants. Cortex, 42, 846–854.
  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.
  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179.
  • Ménard, L., Davis, B. L., Boë, L. J., & Roy, J. P. (2009). Producing American English vowels during vocal tract growth: A perceptual categorization study of synthesized vowels. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 1268–1285.
  • Mesgarani, N., Cheung, C., Johnson, K., & Chang, E. F. (2014). Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science, 343, 1006–1010.
  • Moffitt, A. R. (1971). Consonant cue perception by twenty-to twenty-four-week-old infants. Child Development, 717–731.
  • Moisik, S. R., & Esling, J. H. (2011). The ‘whole larynx’ approach to laryngeal features. Proceedings of ICPhS. Retrieved from https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2011/OnlineProceedings/RegularSession/Moisik/Moisik.pdf
  • Moran, G., Krupka, A., Tutton, A., & Symons, D. (1987). Patterns of maternal and infant imitation during play. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 477–491.
  • Morse, P. A. (1972). The discrimination of speech and nonspeech stimuli in early infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 477–492.
  • Möttönen, R., & Watkins, K. E. (2009). Motor representations of articulators contribute to categorical perception of speech sounds. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9819–9825.
  • Mugitani, R., Kobayashi, T., & Hiraki, K. (2008). Audiovisual matching of lips and non-canonical sounds in 8-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 307–310.
  • Nam, H., Goldstein, L., & Saltzman, E. (2009). Self-organization of syllable structure: A coupled oscillator model. In F. Pellegrino, E. Marsico, I. Chitoran, & C. Coupé (Eds.), Approaches to phonological complexity (pp. 299–328). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Oller, D. K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In G. H. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology (Vol. 1, pp. 93–112). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Oller, D. K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (1982). Similarity of babbling in Spanish- and English-learning babies. Journal of Child Language, 9, 565–77.
  • Opie, I. (1951). Humpty-Dumpty. In I. Opie, & P. Opie (Eds.), The Oxford English dictionary of nursery rhymes (pp. 213–215). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
  • Oudeyer, P. Y. (2006). Self-organization in the evolution of speech. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants. First Language, 9, 137–158.
  • Pegg, J. E., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Adult and infant perception of two English phones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 3742–3753.
  • Pierrehumbert, J. (2003). Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology. Language and Speech, 46, 115–154.
  • Polka, L., Colantonio, C., & Sundara, M. (2001). A cross-language comparison of /d/-/ð/ perception: Evidence for a new developmental pattern. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 2190–2201.
  • Pons, F., Lewkowicz, D. J., Soto-Faraco, S., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). Narrowing of intersensory speech perception in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 10598–10602.
  • Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 188–194.
  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.
  • Rogalsky, C., Love, T., Driscoll, D., Anderson, S. W., & Hickok, G. (2011). Are mirror neurons the basis of speech perception? Evidence from five cases with damage to the purported human mirror system. Neurocase, 17, 178–187.
  • Rosenblum, L. D., Schmuckler, M. A., & Johnson, J. A. (1997). The McGurk effect in infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 347–357.
  • Roug, L., Landberg, I., & Lundberg, L. J. (1989). Phonetic development in early infancy: A study of four Swedish children during the first eighteen months of life. Journal of Child Language, 16, 19–40.
  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.
  • Sams, M., Möttönen, R., & Sihvonen, T. (2005). Seeing and hearing others and oneself talk. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 429–435.
  • Sato, M., Troille, E., Ménard, L., Cathiard, M. A., & Gracco, V. (2013). Silent articulation modulates auditory and audiovisual speech perception. Experimental Brain Research, 227, 275–288.
  • Saxe, J. G. (1873). The blind men and the elephant. In J. G. Saxe, The poems of John Godfrey Saxe (pp. 135–136). Boston MA: James R. Osgood.
  • Shafer, V. L., Yan, H. Y., & Datta, H. (2011). The development of English vowel perception in monolingual and bilingual infants: Neurophysiological correlates. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 527–545.
  • Stark, R. (1980). Prespeech segmental feature development. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. Kavanaugh, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology (Vol. 1, pp. 73–92). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Stockman, I. J., Woods, D. R., & Tishman, A. (1981). Listener agreement on phonetic segments in early infant vocalizations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10, 593–617.
  • Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T. (2000). Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport, 11, 2289–2292.
  • Streeter, L. L. (1976). Language perception of two-month-old infants shows effects of both innate mechanisms and experience. Nature, 259, 39–41.
  • Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1998). The particulate origins of language generativity: From syllable to gesture. In J. R. Hurford, J. R. M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases (pp. 202–221). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Studdert-Kennedy, M. (2002). Mirror neurons, vocal imitation and the evolution of particulate speech. In M. I. Stamenov, & V. Gallese (Eds.), Mirror neurons and the evolution of brain and language (pp. 207–227). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
  • Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Goldstein, L. (2003). Launching language: The gestural origin of discrete infinity. In M. Christiansen, & S. Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 235–254). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Goodell, E. W. (1995). Gestures, features and segments in early child speech. In B. de Gelder, & J. Morais (Eds.), Speech and reading: A comparative approach (pp. 65–88). East Sussex, UK: Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis.
  • Sundara, M., Namasivayam, A. K., & Chen, R. (2001). Observation-execution matching system for speech: A magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport, 12, 1341–1344.
  • Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Genesee, F. (2006). Language-experience facilitates discrimination of /d-/ in monolingual and bilingual acquisition of English. Cognition, 100, 369–388.
  • Sundara, M., & Scutellaro, A. (2011). Rhythmic distance between languages affects the development of speech perception in bilingual infants. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 505–513.
  • Swingley, D. (2005). Statistical clustering and the contents of the infant vocabulary. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 86–132.
  • Tees, R. C., & Werker, J. F. (1984). Perceptual flexibility: Maintenance or recovery of the ability to discriminate non-native speech sounds. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 38, 579[COMP]: Tees & Werker 1984 not in text. Add to text or delete from references.
  • Thevenin, D. M., Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & Lavoie, L. (1985). Where's the drift in babbling drift? A cross-linguistic study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 3–15.
  • Trehub, S. E. (1976). The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 47, 466–472.
  • Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2006). Perception of native and non-native affricate-fricative contrasts: Cross-language tests on adults and infants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 2285–2294.
  • Tyler, M. D., Best, C. T., Avesani, C., Bohn, O.-S., & Vayra, M. (2016). Perception of non-native guttural fricative place contrasts as perceived by native listeners of languages that differ in use of gutturals: English, Danish and Italian. Manuscript in preparation.
  • Tyler, M. D., Best, C. T., Goldstein, L. M., & Antoniou, M. (2014). Contributions of native-language tuning and articulatory organs to infants' discrimination of native and nonnative consonant contrasts. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 210–227.
  • Velleman, S. L., & Vihman, M. M. (2007). Phonology in infancy and early childhood: Implications for theories of language learning. In M. Pennington (Ed.), Phonology in context (pp. 25–50). London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Vihman, M. M. (2002). Getting started without a system: From phonetics to phonology in bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 239–254.
  • Vihman, M., & Croft, W. (2007). Phonological development: Toward a “radical” templatic phonology. Linguistics, 45, 683–725.
  • Vihman, M., DePaolis, R. A., & Keren-Portnoy, T. (2009). A dynamic systems approach to babbling and words. In E. L. Bavin (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 163–182). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J. (1985). From babbling to speech: A re-assessment of the continuity issue. Language, 61, 397–445.
  • Vitevitch, M. S., Armbrüster, J., & Chu, S. (2004). Sublexical and lexical representations in speech production: Effects of phonotactic probability and onset density. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 514.
  • Watkins, K. E., & Paus, T. (2004). Modulation of motor excitability during speech perception: The role of Broca's area. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 978–987.
  • Watkins, K. E., Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T. (2003). Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia, 41, 989–994.
  • Wedel, A. (2004). Category competition drives contrast maintenance within an exemplar-based production/perception loop. Proceedings of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Current themes in computational phonology and morphology (pp. 1–10). Stroudsburg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics.
  • Weikum, W. M., Oberlander, T. F., Hensch, T. K., & Werker, J. F. (2012). Prenatal exposure to antidepressants and depressed maternal mood alter trajectory of infant speech perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(Suppl. 2), 17221–17227.
  • Werker, J. F. (1989). On becoming a native listener. American Scientist, 77, 54–59.
  • Werker, J. F. (1991). The ontogeny of speech perception. In I. G. Mattingly, & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Modularity and the Motor Theory of speech perception (pp. 91–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Werker, J. F., & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1, 197–234.
  • Werker, J. F., & Lalonde, C. E. (1988). Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology, 24, 672–683.
  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63.
  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1992). The organization and reorganization of human speech perception. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 377–402.
  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1999). Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 509–535.
  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (2005). Speech perception as a window for understanding plasticity and commitment in language systems of the brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 46(3), 233–251.
  • Whalen, D. H., Levitt, A. G., & Goldstein, L. (2007). VOT in the babbling of French- and English-learning infants. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 341–352.
  • Whalen, D. H., Levitt, A. G., & Wang, Q. (1991). Intonational differences between the reduplicative babbling of French- and English-learning infants. Journal of Child Language, 18, 501–516.
  • Yeung, H. H., & Werker, J. F. (2013). Lip movements affect infants' audiovisual speech perception. Psychological Science, 24, 603–612.
  • Yoshida, K. A., Pons, F., Maye, J., & Werker, J. F. (2010). Distributional phonetic learning at 10 months of age. Infancy, 15, 420–433.