731
Views
23
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Useful Dimensions of Haptic Perception: 50 Years After The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems

&

References

  • Abdolvahab, M., Carello, C., Pinto, C., Turvey, M. T., & Frank, T. D. (2015). Symmetry and order parameter dynamics of the human odometer. Biological Cybernetics, 109, 63–73.
  • Amazeen, E. L. (1999). Perceptual independence of size and weight by dynamic touch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 102–119.
  • Amazeen, E. L., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). Weight perception and the haptic size-weight illusion are functions of the inertia tensor. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 213–232.
  • Amazeen, P., Amazeen, E. L., & Turvey, M. T. (1998). Dynamics of human intersegmental coordination: Theory and research. In C. Collyer & D. Rosenbaum (Eds.), Timing of behavior: Neural, psychological and computational perspectives (pp. 237–260). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Berkeley, G. (1709). Essay toward a new theory of vision. Dublin, Ireland: Aaron Rhames for Jeremy Pepyat.
  • Bernstein, N. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.
  • Bernstein, N. (1996). On dexterity and its development. In M. L. Latash & M. T. Turvey (Eds.), Dexterity and its development (pp. 3–244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Bingham, G. P. (1993). Perceiving the size of trees: Form as information about scale. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 1139–1161.
  • Bingham, G. P., Schmidt, R. C., & Rosenblum, L. D. (1989). Hefting for a maximum distance throw: A smart perceptual mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 507–528.
  • Boring, E. G. (1942). Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. New York, NY: Appleton Century Crofts.
  • Burton, G. (1992). Nonvisual judgment of the crossability of path gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 698–713.
  • Cabe, P. (2010). Sufficiency of longitudinal moment of inertia for haptic cylinder length judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 373–394.
  • Carello, C. (2004). Perceiving affordances by dynamic touch: Hints from the control of movement. Ecological Psychology, 16, 31–36.
  • Carello, C., Fitzpatrick, P., Domaniewicz, I., Chan, T.-C., & Turvey, M. T. (1992). Effortful touch with minimal movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 290–302.
  • Carello, C., Fitzpatrick, P., Flascher, I., & Turvey, M. T. (1998). Inertial eigenvalues, rod density, and rod diameter in length perception by dynamic touch. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 89–100.
  • Carello, C., Flascher, I., Kunkler-Peck, A., & Turvey, M. T. (1999). An evaluation of context effects in dynamic touch. Ecological Psychology, 11, 194–207.
  • Carello, C., Kinsella-Shaw, J., Amazeen, E. L., & Turvey, M. T. (2006). Peripheral neuropathy and object length perception by effortful (dynamic) touch: A case study. Neuroscience Letters, 405, 159–163.
  • Carello, C., Silva, P. L., Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., & Turvey, M. T. (2009). Sensory and motor challenges to muscle-based perception. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy, 12, 339–350.
  • Carello, C., Thuot, S., Andersen, K. L., & Turvey, M. T. (1999). Perceiving the sweet spot. Perception, 28, 307–320.
  • Carello, C., Thuot, S., & Turvey, M. T. (2000). Aging and the perception of a racket's sweet spot. Human Movement Science, 19, 1–20.
  • Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2000). Rotational invariants and dynamic touch. In M. Heller (Ed.), Touch, representation and blindness (pp. 27–66). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2004). Physics and psychology of the muscle sense. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 25–28.
  • Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2015). Dynamic (effortful) touch. In T. J. Prescott, E. Ahissar, & E. Izhikevich (Eds.), Scholarpedia of touch (pp. 229–242). Paris, France: Atlantis Press.
  • Carello, C., & Wagman, J. B. (2009). Mutuality in the perception of affordances and the control of movement. In D. Sternad (Ed.), Progress in motor control (pp. 273–292). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  • Cole, J. (1995). Pride and a daily marathon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Cooper, M., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1999). Further evidence of perceptual independencies (specificities) in dynamic touch. Ecological Psychology, 11, 269–281.
  • Cooper, M., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2000). Perceptual independence of whole length, partial length, and hand position in wielding a rod. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 74–85.
  • Feldman, A. G. (2015). Referent control of action and perception: Challenging conventional theories in behavioral neuroscience. New York, NY: Springer.
  • Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., Schmidt, R. C., & Corey, D. (1994). Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecological Psychology, 6, 265–287.
  • Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1994). Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscle sense.” Neuroscience, 60, 551–568.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychological Review, 69, 477–490.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1963). The useful dimensions of sensitivity. American Psychologist, 18, 1–15.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Original work published 1979).
  • Gogel, W. C. (1977). The metric of visual space. In W. Epstein (Ed.), Stability and constancy in visual perception (pp. 129–181). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Golubitsky, M., Stewart, I., Buono, P.-L., & Collins, J. J. (1999). Symmetry in locomotor central pattern generators and animal gaits. Nature, 401, 693–695.
  • Hajnal, A., Fonseca, S. T., Harrison, S., Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., & Carello, C. (2007). Comparison of dynamic (effortful) touch by hand and foot. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 82–88.
  • Hajnal, A., Fonseca, S., Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., Silva, P., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2007). Haptic selective attention by foot and by hand. Neuroscience Letters, 419, 5–9.
  • Hajnal, A., Wagman, J. B., Doyon, J. K., & Clark, J. D. (2016). Perception of stand-on-ability: Do geographical slants feel steeper than they look? Perception, 45, 768–786.
  • Harré, R. (2002). Great scientific experiments: Twenty experiments that changed our view of the world. New York, NY: Dover.
  • Hove, P., Riley, M. A., & Shockley, K. (2006). Perceiving affordances of hockey sticks by dynamic touch. Ecological Psychology, 18, 163–189.
  • Huijing, P. A. (2003). Muscular force transmission necessitates a multilevel integrative approach to the analysis of function of skeletal muscle. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 31, 167–175.
  • Huijing, P. A., & Baan, G. C. (2001). Extramuscular myofascial force transmission within the rat anterior tibial compartment: Proximo-distal differences in muscle force. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 173, 297–311.
  • Huijing, P. A., & Baan, G. C. (2003). Myofascial force transmission: Muscle relative position and length determine agonist and synergist muscle force. Journal of Applied Physiology, 94, 1092–1107.
  • Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 718–727.
  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kingma, I., van de Langenberg, R., & Beek, P. J. (2004). Which mechanical invariants are associated with the perception of length and heaviness of a nonvisible handheld rod? Testing the inertia tensor hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 346–354.
  • Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., Shaw, B., & Turvey, M. T. (1992). Perceiving walk-on-able slopes. Ecological Psychology, 4, 65–95.
  • Klatzky, R. L., & Reed, C. L. (2015). Haptic exploration. Scholarpedia, 4, 7941.
  • Koike, T., Miyamoto, E., & Tamagaki, T. (2007). Dynamic touch in patients with cervical spinal cord injury. In S. Cummins-Sebree, M. A. Riley, & K. Shockley (Eds.), Studies in perception & action IX (pp. 151–154). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Kugler, P. N., & Turvey, M. T. (1987). Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Langevin, H. M., & Huijing, P. A. (2009). Communicating about fascia: History, pitfalls, and recommendations. International Journal of Therapeutic Massage and Bodywork, 2, 3–8.
  • Lawson, R. (2014). Recognizing familiar objects by hand and foot: Haptic shape perception generalizes to inputs from unusual locations and untrained body parts. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 541–558.
  • Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.
  • Lee, D. N. (1978). The functions of vision. In H. L. Pick & E. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information (pp. 159–170). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Lee, D. N. (1980). Visuo-motor coordination in space-time. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 281–295). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.
  • Malek, E. A., & Wagman, J. B. (2008). Kinetic potential influences visual and remote haptic perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1813–1826.
  • Millikan, R. (1984). Language, thought and other biological categories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Millikan, R. (1993). White queen psychology and other essays for Alice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Müller, J. M. (1965). On the specific energies of nerves. In R. Herrnstein & E. Boring (Eds.), A sourcebook of the history of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1838)
  • Pagano, C., Fitzpatrick, P., & Turvey, M. T. (1993). Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 43–45.
  • Pagano, C., Garrett, S., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). Is limb proprioception a function of the limbs' inertial eigenvectors? Ecological Psychology, 8, 43–69.
  • Pagano, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1993). Perceiving by dynamic touch the distances reachable with irregular objects. Ecological Psychology, 5, 125–151.
  • Pagano, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1995). The inertia tensor as a basis for the perception of limb orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1070–1087.
  • Palatinus, Z., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2011). Principles of part-whole selective perception by dynamic touch extend to the torso. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43, 87–94.
  • Palatinus, Z., Kelty-Stephen, D., Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2014). Haptic intent in quiet standing affects multifractal scaling of postural fluctuations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1808–1818.
  • Prescott, T. J., Ahissar, E., & Izhikevich, E. (2016). Scholarpedia of touch. Paris, France: Atlantis Press.
  • Prescott, T. J., & Dürr, V. (2015). The world of touch. Scholarpedia, 10, 32688.
  • Peck, A., Jeffers, R. G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). Haptically perceiving the length of one rod by means of another. Ecological Psychology, 8, 237–258.
  • Rack, P. M., & Westbury, D. R. (1969). The effects of length and stimulus rate on tension in the isometric cat soleus muscle. Journal of Physiology, 204, 443–460.
  • Reed, E. S. (1988). James J. Gibson and the psychology of perception. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  • Regia-Corte, T., & Wagman, J. B. (2008). Perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface depends on height of center of mass. Experimental Brain Research, 191, 25–35.
  • Riley, M. A., & Van Orden, G. (Eds.). (2005). Tutorials in contemporary nonlinear methods for the behavioral sciences. Retrieved from http://www.nsf.gov/sbe/bcs/pac/nmbs/nmbs.jsp
  • Riley, M. A., Wagman, J. B., Santana, M.-V., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2002). Perceptual behavior: Recurrence analysis of a haptic exploratory procedure. Perception, 31, 481–510.
  • Runeson, S. (1977). On the possibility of smart perceptual mechanisms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 18, 172–179.
  • Santana, M.-V., & Carello, C. (1999). Perception by dynamic touch at the small scale. Ecological Psychology, 11, 283–307.
  • Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 227–247.
  • Schwartz, M. (1999). Haptic perception of the distance walked when blindfolded. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 852–865.
  • Shaw, R. E. (2001). Processes, acts, and experiences: Three stances on the problem of intentionality. Ecological Psychology, 13, 275–314.
  • Shaw, R. E., Turvey, M. T., & Mace, W. M. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes II (pp. 159–226). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Shockley, K., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2004). Metamers in the haptic perception of heaviness and moveable–ness. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 731–742.
  • Shockley, K., Grocki, M., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2001). Somatosensory attunement to the rigid body laws. Experimental Brain Research, 136, 133–137.
  • Silva, P. L., Moreno, M., Mancini, M., Fonseca, S. T., & Turvey, M. T. (2007). Steady-state stress at one hand magnifies the amplitude, stiffness, and non-linearity of oscillatory behavior at the other hand. Neuroscience Letters, 429, 64–68.
  • Silva, P. L., & Turvey, M. T. (2012). The role of haptic information in shaping coordination dynamics: Inertial frame of reference hypothesis. Human Movement Science, 31, 1014–1036.
  • Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 404–427.
  • Solomon, H. Y., Turvey, M. T., & Burton, G. (1989). Gravitational and muscular contributions to perceiving extent by wielding. Ecological Psychology, 1, 265–300.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., & Bardy, B. G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 195–261.
  • Turvey, M. T., Burton, G., Amazeen, E. L., Butwill, M., & Carello, C. (1998). Perceiving the width and height of a hand-held object by dynamic touch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 35–48.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (1995). Dynamic touch. In W. Epstein & S. Rogers (Eds.), Handbook of perception and cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of space and motion (pp. 401–490). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (2011). Obtaining information by dynamic (effortful) touching. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 3123–3132.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Fonseca, S. (2009). Nature of motor control: Perspectives and issues. In D. Sternad (Ed.), Progress in motor control: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 93–123). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Fonseca, S. T. (2014). The medium of haptic perception: A tensegrity hypothesis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 46, 143–187.
  • Turvey, M. T., Harrison, S. J., Frank, T. D., & Carello, C. (2012). Human odometry verifies the symmetry perspective on bipedal gaits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1014–1025.
  • Turvey, M. T., Romaniak-Gross, C., Isenhower, R. W., Arzamarski, R., Harrison, S., & Carello, C. (2009). Human odometry is gait-symmetry specific. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276, 4309–4314.
  • Turvey, M. T., Shockley, K., & Carello, C. (1999). Affordance, proper function, and the physical basis of perceived heaviness. Cognition, 73, B17–B26.
  • Turvey, M. T., Solomon, H. Y., & Burton, G. (1989). An ecological analysis of knowing by wielding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 387–407.
  • van de Langenberg, R., Kingma, I., & Beek, P. J. (2007). Perception of limb orientation in the vertical plane depends on center of mass rather than inertial eigenvectors. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 595–607.
  • van de Langenberg, R., Kingma, I., & Beek, P. J. (2008). The perception of limb orientation depends on the center of mass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 624–639.
  • Wagman, J. B. (2015). As easy to move as a feather: Perception of “lightness” as “ease to move.” Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 340–342.
  • Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Inertial constraints on affordances of tools. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–195.
  • Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: Grasping so as to create functional objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 175–186.
  • Wagman, J. B., Carello, C., Schmidt, R. C., & Turvey, M. T. (2009). Is perceptual learning unimodal? Ecological Psychology, 21, 37–67.
  • Wagman, J. B., & Hajnal, A. (2014). Task-specificity and anatomical independence in perception of surface properties by means of a wielded object. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 40, 2372–2391.
  • Wagman, J. B., & Hajnal, A. (2016). Use your head! Perception of action possibilities by means of an object attached to the head. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 829–836.
  • Wagman, J. B., & Shockley, K. (2011). Metamers for hammer-with-ability are not metamers for poke-with-ability. Ecological Psychology, 23, 76–92.
  • Wagman, J. B., & Taylor, K. R. (2004). Chosen striking location and the user-tool-environment system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 267–280.
  • Wagman, J. B., & Taylor, K. R. (2005). Perceiving affordances for aperture crossing for the person-plus-object system. Ecological Psychology, 17, 105–130.
  • Wagman, J. B., Zimmerman, C., & Sorric, C. (2007). Which feels heavier—a pound of lead or a pound of feathers? A potential perceptual basis of a cognitive riddle. Perception, 36, 1709–1711.
  • Webber, C. L., Jr., & Zbilut, J. P. (1994). Dynamical assessment of physiological systems and states using recurrence plot strategies. Journal of Applied Physiology, 76, 956–973.
  • Webber, C. L., Jr., & Zbilut, J. P. (1996). Assessing deterministic structures in physiological systems using recurrence plot strategies. In M. C. K. Khoo (Ed.), Bioengineering approaches to pulmonary physiology and medicine (pp. 137–148). New York, NY: Plenum.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.