405
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

An Emerging Developmental Ecological Psychology: Future Directions and Potentials

&

References

  • Baedke, J. (2013). The epigenetic landscape in the course of time: Conrad Hal Waddington's methodological impact on the life sciences. Studies in History and Philosophy of science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 44, 756–773. doi:10.1016/j.shpsc.2013.06.001
  • Darwin, C. (1869). On the origin of species by means of natural selection (5th ed.). London, UK: John Murray.
  • Dent, C., & Zukow, P. (Eds.). (1990). The idea of innateness: Effects on language and communication research [Special issue]. Developmental Biology, 23.
  • Dewey, J. (1910). The influence of Darwin on philosophy and other essays in contemporary thought. New York, NY: Harry Holt.
  • Dewey, J. (1976). Lectures on psychological and political ethics, 1898 (D. L. Koch, Ed.). New York, NY: Hafner Press.
  • French, V., Bryant, P. J., & Bryant, S. V. (1976). Pattern regulation in epimorphic fields. Science, 193, 969–981. doi:10.1126/science.948762
  • Furst, B. (2014). The heart and circulation: An integrative model. London, UK: Springer.
  • Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  • Gibson, E. J. (1997). An ecological psychologist's prolegomena for perceptual development: A functional approach. In C. E. Dent-Read & P. E. Zukow-Goldring (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development: Ecological approaches to organism–environment systems (pp. 23–54). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Gibson, E. J. (2003a). The world is so full of a number of things: On specification and perceptual learning. Ecological Psychology, 15, 283–289. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco1504_3
  • Gibson, E. J. (2003b). What psychology is about: Ruminations of an opinionated aged psychologist. Ecological Psychology, 15, 289–297. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco1504_4
  • Gibson, E. J., & Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Original work published 1979) Fix
  • Gilbert, S. F., & Sarkar, S. (2000). Embracing complexity: Organicism for the 21st century. Developmental Dynamics, 219, 1–9. doi:10.1002/1097-0177(2000)9999:9999%3c::AID-DVDY1036%3e3.0.CO;2-A
  • Goethe, W. V. (1988). Goethe: Scientific studies, chapter II, morphology (D. Miller, Trans., pp. 53–73). New York, NY: Suhrkamp.
  • Goodwin, B. (1978). A cognitive view of biological process. Journal of Social and Biological Systems, 1, 117–125. doi:10.1016/S0140-1750(78)80001-3
  • Goodwin, B. (1979). On morphogenetic fields. Theoria to Theory, 13, 109–114.
  • Goodwin, B. (1982). Development and evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 97, 43–55. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(82)90275-2
  • Goodwin, B. (1990). Causes of development in biology and ethology. In G. Butterworth & P. Bryant (Eds.), Causes of development (pp. 49–63). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Goodwin, B., & Saunders, P. (Eds.). (1989). Theoretical biology: Epigenetic and evolutionary order from complex systems. Baltimore, MD, and London, UK: Johns Hopkins University Press.
  • Gottlieb, G. (2003). Probabilistic epigenesist of development. In J. Valsiner & K. Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 3–18). London, UK: Sage.
  • Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 1–11. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x
  • Grene, M. (1974). The understanding of nature: Essays in the philosophy of biology (Vol. 23). Boston, MA: D. Reidel.
  • Grene, M. G. (1995). A philosophical testament. Chicago, IL: Open Court.
  • Haraway, D. J. (1976). Crystals, fabrics, and fields: Metaphors that shape embryos. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books.
  • Johnston, T., & Gottlieb, G. (1987). Neophenogenesis: A developmental theory of phenotypic evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 147, 471–495. doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80260-7
  • Keller, E. F. (2010). The mirage of a space between nature and nurture. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
  • Kelty-Stephen, D., & Dixon, J. A. (2012). When physics is not “just physics” Complexity science invites new measurement frames for exploring the physics of cognitive and biological development. Critical Review in Biomedical Engineering, 40, 471–483. doi:10.1615/CritRevBiomedEng.2013006693
  • Koonin, E. F. (2009). Darwinian evolution in the light of genomics. Nucleac Acids Research, 37, 1011–1034. doi:10.1093/nar/gkp089
  • Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., & Odling-Smee, J. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 282, 1–14. doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.1019
  • Lenoir, T. (1987). The eternal laws of form: Morphotypes and the conditions of existence in Goethe's biological thought. In F. Amrine, F. Zucker, & H. Wheeler (Eds.), Goethe and the sciences: A reappraisal (pp. 17–29). Boston, MA: D. Reidel.
  • Lewontin, R. (2002). The triple helix: Gene, organism, environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • MacDonald, N. (1977). Time lag in a model of a biochemical reaction sequence with end product inhibition. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 67, 549–556. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(77)90056-X
  • Margolis, J. (1987). Goethe and psychoanalysis. In F. Amrine, F. Zucker, & H. Wheeler (Eds.), Goethe and the sciences: A reappraisal (pp. 83–101). Boston, MA: D. Reidel.
  • Mathews, J., & Levin, M. (2017). Gap junctional signaling in pattern regulation: Physiological network connectivity instructs growth and form. Developmental Neurobiology, 77, 643–673. doi:10.1002/dneu.22405
  • Michaels, C. F., & Palatinus, Z. (2014). A ten commandments for ecological psychology. In L. Shapiro (Ed.), Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 19–28). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
  • Miller, D., & Wagman, J. (2003). Nested reciprocities: The organism-environment system in perception-action and development. Developmental Psychobiology, 42, 317–334. doi:10.1002/dev.10114
  • Oyama, S. (1985, 2000). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Pigliucci, M., & Müller, G. B. (2010). Evolution: The extended synthesis. Boston, MA: MIT Press.
  • Pittenger, J. B., & Dent, C. H. (1988). A mechanism for the direct perception of change: The example of bacterial chemotaxis. Perception, 17, 119–133. doi:10.1068/p170119
  • Rader, N., Spiro, D., & Firestone, P. (1979). Performance on a stage IV object-permanence task with standard and nonstandard covers. Child Development, 50, 908–910. doi:10.2307/1128966
  • Rader, N., & Vaughn, L. A. (2000). Infant reaching to a hidden affordance: Evidence for intentionality. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 531–541. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00060-1
  • Rader, N., & Zukow-Goldring, P. (2010). How the hands control attention during early word learning. Gesture, 10, 202–221.
  • Rader, N., & Zukow-Goldring, P. (2012). Caregivers' gestures direct infant attention during early word learning: The importance of dynamic synchrony. Language Sciences, 34, 559–568. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.011
  • Raeff, C. (2011). Distinguishing between development and change: Reviving organismic-developmental theory. Human Development, 54, 4–33. doi:10.1159/000324334
  • Read, C., & Szokolszky, A. (2016). A developmental ecological study of novel metaphoric language use. Language Sciences, 53, 86–98. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2015.07.003
  • Reid, T. (1863). The works of Thomas Reid (6th ed.). (W. M. Hamilton, Ed., pp. 715–719). Edinburgh, UK: Maclachlan and Stewart.
  • Rietveld, E., & Paglieri, F. (2012). Bodily intentionality and social affordances in context: The role of the natural and social context in shaping consciousness. Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins.
  • Runeson, S. (1977). On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 18, 172–179. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00274.x
  • Shapiro, J. A. (2011). Evolution: A view from the 21st century. Upper Saddle River, NJ: FT Press Science.
  • Szokolszky, A., & Read, C. (2018). Developmental ecological psychology and a coalition of ecological-relatioal developmental approaches. Ecological Psychology, 30, 6–38.
  • Szokolszky, A. (2003). An interview with Eleanor Gibson. Ecological Psychology, 15, 271–281. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco1504_2
  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (1996). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Thelen, E., & Smith, B. (2006). Dynamic systems theories. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., pp. 258–312). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Turvey, M. (2009). On the notion and implications of organism-environment system: Introduction. Ecological Psychology, 21, 97–112. doi:10.1080/10407410902877041
  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (2012a). On intelligence from first principles: Guidelines for inquiry into the hypothesis of physical intelligence (PI). Ecological Psychology, 24, 3–32. doi:10.1080/10407413.2012.645757
  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (2012b). On intelligence from first principles: Part II. Ecological Psychology, 24, 179–185. doi:10.1080/10407413.2012.702610
  • Turvey, M., & Sheya, A. (2017). Non-obvious influences on perception-action abilities. Psychonomic Bulletin Review. 24, 1597–1603. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1223-2
  • Waddington, C. H. (1942). Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature, 150, 563–565. doi:10.1038/150563a0
  • Waddington, C. H. (1957). The strategy of the genes. London, UK: Routledge, Taylor and Francis Group.
  • Webster, G., & Goodwin, B. (1996). Form and transformation: Generative and relational principles in biology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Wilcox, S., & Katz, S. (1981a). The ecological approach to development: An alternative to cognitivism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 247–263. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(81)90079-5
  • Wilcox, S., & Katz, S. (1981b). A direct realist alternative to the traditional conception of memory. Behaviorism, 9, 227–239.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.