441
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Opinion Versus Knowledge: The Influence of Testimony Format on Children’s Judgments in Morally Relevant Contexts

, , , &

References

  • Birch, S. J., Vauthier, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2009). Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition, 107(3), 1018–1034. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008
  • Boseovski, J. J. (2012). Trust in testimony about strangers: Young children prefer reliable informants who make positive attributions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 543–551. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.10.008
  • Boseovski, J. J., & Thurman, S. L. (2014). Evaluating and approaching a strange animal: Children’s trust in informant testimony. Child Development, 85(2), 824–834. doi:10.1111/cdev.12156
  • Chen, E. E., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2013). Children trust a consensus composed of outgroup members—but do not retain that trust. Child Development, 84, 269–282. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01850.x
  • Clément, F., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. (2004). The ontogenesis of trust. Mind & Language, 19, 360–379. doi:10.1111/j.0268-1064.2004.00263.x
  • Cohen, G. L., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Peer contagion of aggression and health risk behavior among adolescent males: An experimental investigation of effects on public conduct and private attitudes. Child Development, 77, 967–983. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00913.x
  • Corriveau, K. H., Kim, E., Song, G., & Harris, P. L. (2013). Young children’s deference to a consensus varies by culture and judgment setting. Journal of Cognition And Culture, 13, 367–381. doi:10.1163/15685373-12342099
  • Einav, S. (2014). Does the majority always know the best? Young children’s flexible trust in majority opinion. PLoS ONE, 9, 1–5. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104585
  • Fu, G., Xiao, W. S., Killen, M., & Lee, K. (2014). Moral judgment and its relation to second-order theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2085–2092. doi:10.1037/a0037077
  • Fusaro, M., & Harris, P. L. (2008). Children assess informant reliability using bystanders’ non-verbal cues. Developmental Science, 11, 771–777. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00728.x
  • Guerrero, S., Elenbaas, L., Enesco, I., & Killen, M. (in press). Preschoolers’ trust in social consensus varies by context: Conventional versus moral domains. Anales De Psicología.
  • Haun, D. M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Development, 82, 1759–1767. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01666.x
  • Heiphetz, L., Spelke, E. S., Harris, P. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). The development of reasoning about beliefs: Fact, preference, and ideology. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 559–565. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.09.005
  • Heiphetz, L., Spelke, E. S., Harris, P. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2014). What do different beliefs tell us? An examination of factual, opinion-based, and religious beliefs. Cognitive Development, 30, 15–29. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.12.002
  • Jaswal, V. K. (2004). Don’t believe everything you hear: Preschoolers’ sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development, 75, 1871–1885. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00822.x
  • Jaswal, V. K., Carrington Croft, A., Setia, A. R., & Cole, C. A. (2010). Young children have a specific, highly robust bias to trust testimony. Psychological Science, 21, 1541–1547. doi:10.1177/0956797610383438
  • Jaswal, V. K., & Neely, L. A. (2006). Adults don’t always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17(9), 757–758. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x
  • Kebbell, M. R., & Milne, R. (1998). Police officers’ perceptions of eyewitness performance in forensic investigations. Journal of Social Psychology, 138, 323–330. doi:10.1080/00224549809600384
  • Killen, M., Mulvey, K. L., Richardson, C., Jampol, N., & Woodward, A. (2011). The accidental transgressor: Morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition, 119, 197–215. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.006
  • Koenig, M. A., Clément, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15, 694–698. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x
  • Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261–1277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
  • Lane, J. D., Harris, P. L., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). More than meets the eye: Young children’s trust in claims that defy their perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 50, 865–871. doi:10.1037/a0034291
  • Leslie, A. M., Knobe, J., & Cohen, A. (2006). Acting intentionally and the side-effect effect: Theory of mind and moral judgment. Psychological science, 17(5), 421–427. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01722.x
  • McGlothlin, H., & Killen, M. (2006). Intergroup attitudes of European American children attending ethnically homogeneous schools. Child Development, 77, 1375–1386. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00941.x
  • Memon, A., Gabbert, F., & Hope, L. (2004). The ageing eyewitness. In J. R. Adler (Ed.), Forensic psychology: Concepts, debates and practice (pp. 96–112). Devon, UK: Willan.
  • Pasquini, E. S., Corriveau, K. H., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2007). Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1216–1226. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216
  • Pillow, B. H. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 116–129. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(89)90066-0
  • Pratt, C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Young children understand that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel). Child Development, 61, 973–982. doi:10.2307/1130869
  • Shi, B., & Xie, H. (2012). Socialization of physical and social aggression in early adolescents’ peer groups: High‐status peers, individual status, and gender. Social Development, 21, 170–194. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00621.x
  • Smetana, J. G., Jambon, M., & Ball, C. (2014). The social domain approach to children’s moral and social judgments. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 23–45). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Taylor, M., Cartwright, B. S., & Bowden, T. (1991). Perspective taking and theory of mind: Do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers’ knowledge? Child Development, 62, 1334–1351. doi:10.2307/1130810
  • VanderBorght, M., & Jaswal, V. K. (2009). Who knows best? Preschoolers sometimes prefer child informants over adult informants. Infant and Child Development, 18, 61–71. doi:10.1002/icd.591
  • Walker, M. B., & Andrade, M. G. (1996). Conformity in the Asch task as a function of age. Journal of Social Psychology, 136, 367–372. doi:10.1080/00224545.1996.9714014
  • Whitcombe, E. L., & Robinson, E. J. (2000). Children’s decisions about what to believe and their ability to report the source of their belief. Cognitive Development, 15, 329–346. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00033-2
  • Wójcik, M., & Kozak, B. (2015). Bullying and exclusion from dominant peer group in polish middle schools. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 46, 2–14. doi:10.1515/ppb-2015-0001

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.