2,631
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Social Cognition, Language, and Prosocial Behaviors: A Multitrait Mixed-Methods Study in Early Childhood

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

References

  • Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (1988). Developing theories of mind. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Baillargeon, R. H., Morisset, A., Keenan, K., Normand, C. L., Jeyaganth, S., Boivin, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2011). The development of prosocial behaviors in young children: A prospective population-based cohort study. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172, 221–251. doi:10.1080/00221325.2010.533719
  • Baker, E., Tisak, M. S., & Tisak, J. (2016). What can boys and girls do? Preschoolers’ perspectives regarding gender roles across domains of behavior. Social Psychology of Education, 19(1), 23–29. doi:10.1007/s11218-015-9320-z
  • Barnett, M. A., Gustafsson, H., Deng, M., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Cox, M. (2012). Bidirectional associations among sensitive parenting, language development, and social competence. Infant & Child Development, 21(4), 374–393. doi:10.1002/icd.1750
  • Bassett, H. H., Denham, S., Mincic, M., & Graling, K. (2012). The structure of preschoolers’ emotion knowledge: Model equivalence and validity using a structural equation modeling approach. Early Education & Development, 23, 259–279. doi:10.1080/10409289.2012.630825
  • Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24(3), 267–304. doi:10.1177/0142723704045681
  • Brownell, C. A., Iesue, S. S., Nichols, S. R., & Svetlova, M. (2013). Mine or yours? Development of sharing in toddlers in relation to ownership understanding. Child Development, 84(3), 906–920. doi:10.1111/cdev.12009
  • Brownell, C. A., Svetlova, M., Anderson, R., Nichols, S. R., & Drummond, J. (2013). Socialization of early prosocial behavior: Parents’ talk about emotions is associated with sharing and helping in toddlers. Infancy, 18(1), 91–119. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00125.x
  • Camodeca, M., & Coppola, G. (2010). Competenza socio-emotiva e difficoltà di relazione in età prescolare: Un approccio multi-informatore [Social-emotional competence and relationship difficulties in preschool years: A multi-informant approach]. In E. Baumgartner (Ed.), Gli esordi della competenza emotiva. Strumenti di studio e di valutazione (pp. 51–74). Milan, Italy: LED Edizioni Universitarie.
  • Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
  • Cassidy, K. W., Werner, R. S., Rourke, M., Zubernis, L. S., & Balaraman, G. (2003). The relationship between psychological understanding and positive social behaviours. Social Development, 12, 198–221. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00229
  • Charman, T., Ruffman, T., & Clements, W. (2002). Is there a gender difference in false belief development? Social Development, 11, 1–10. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00183
  • Cigala, A., Mori, A., & Fangareggi, F. (2015). Learning others’ point of view: Perspective taking and prosocial behaviour in preschoolers. Early Child Development and Care, 185, 1199–1215. doi:10.1080/03004430.2014.987272
  • Curby, T. W., Brock, L. L., & Hamre, B. K. (2013). Teachers’ emotional support consistency predicts children’s achievement gains and social skills. Early Education & Development, 24(3), 292–309. doi:10.1080/10409289.2012.665760
  • de Rosnay, M., Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & Morrell, J. M. B. (2004). A lag between understanding false belief and emotion attribution in young children: Relationships with linguistic ability and mothers’ mental-state language. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 197–218. doi:10.1348/026151004323044573
  • Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194–201. doi:10.2307/1130651
  • Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Miller, S. L. (2017). Early childhood teachers’ socialization of emotion: Contextual and individual contributors. Child Youth Care Forum, 46(6), 805–824. doi:10.1007/s10566-017-9409-y
  • Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., Thayer, S. K., Mincic, M. S., Sirotkin, Y. S., & Zinsser, K. (2012). Observing preschoolers’ social-emotional behavior: Structure, foundations, and prediction of early school success. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 173(3), 246–278. doi:10.1080/00221325.2011.597457
  • Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. M. (2010). Gender differences in the socialization of preschoolers’ emotional competence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 128, 29–49. doi:10.1002/cd.267
  • Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., & Queenan, P. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence? Child Development, 74(1), 238–256. doi:10.1111/cdev.2003.74.issue-1
  • Drummond, J., Hammond, S. I., Satlof-Bedrick, E., Waugh, W. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2017). Helping the one you hurt: Toddlers’ rudimentary guilt, shame, and prosocial behavior after harming another. Child Development, 88(4), 1382–1397. doi:10.1111/cdev.12653
  • Dunfield, K. A. (2014). A construct divided: Prosocial behavior as helping, sharing, and comforting subtypes. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(958), 1–13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00958
  • Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2013). Classifying prosocial behavior: Children’s responses to instrumental need, emotional distress, and material desire. Child Development, 84(5), 1766–1776. doi:10.1111/cdev.12075
  • Dunfield, K., Kuhlmeier, V. A., O’Connell, L., & Kelley, E. (2011). Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior in infancy: Helping, sharing, and comforting. Infancy, 16(3), 227–247. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00041.x
  • Eggum, N. D., Eisenberg, N., Kao, K., Spinrad, T. L., Bolnick, R., Hofer, C., … Fabricius, W. V. (2011). Emotion understanding, theory of mind, and prosocial orientation: Relations over time in early childhood. Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(1), 4–16. doi:10.1080/17439760.2010.536776
  • Eisenberg, N., Eggum-Wilkens, N. D., & Spinrad, T. L. (2015). The development of prosocial behavior. In D. A. Schroeder & W. G. Graziano (Eds.), Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior (pp. 114–136). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 701–778). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2005). More than talk: Relations between emotion understanding and positive behaviour in toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 343–363. doi:10.1348/026151005X26291
  • Ensor, R., Spencer, D., & Hughes, C. (2011). “You feel sad?” Emotion understanding mediates effects of verbal ability and mother–child mutuality on prosocial behaviors: Findings from 2 years to 4 years. Social Development, 20(1), 93–110. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00572.x
  • Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Perez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., … Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 326–343. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x
  • Farrant, B. M., Devine, T. A. J., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2012). Empathy, perspective taking and prosocial behaviour: The importance of parenting practices. Infant and Child Development, 21(2), 175–188. doi:10.1002/icd.740
  • Fox, J. (2008). Applied regression analysis and generalized linear models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Frane, A. (2015). Are per-family type I error rates relevant in social and behavioral science? Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 14(1), 12–23. doi:10.22237/jmasm/1430453040
  • Garner, P. W., Dunsmore, J. C., & Southam-Gerrow, M. (2008). Mother-child conversations about emotions: Linkages to child aggression and prosocial behavior. Social Development, 17, 259–277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00424.x
  • George, D., & Mallery, M. (2010). Using SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple guide and reference. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
  • Girard, L., Pingault, J., Doyle, O., Falissard, B., & Tremblay, R. E. (2017). Expressive language and prosocial behaviour in early childhood: Longitudinal associations in the UK Millennium Cohort Study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14(4), 381–398. doi:10.1080/17405629.2016.1215300
  • Grazzani, I., Ornaghi, V., Agliati, A., & Brazzelli, E. (2016). How to foster toddlers’ mental-state talk, emotion understanding, and prosocial behavior: A conversation-based intervention at nursery school. Infancy, 21(2), 199–227. doi:10.1111/infa.12107
  • Grazzani, I., Ornaghi, V., & Brockmeier, J. (2016). Conversation on mental states at nursery: Promoting social cognition in early childhood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(5), 563–581. doi:10.1080/17405629.2015.1127803
  • Gross, R. L., Drummond, J., Satlof-Bedrick, E., Waugh, W. E., Svetlova, M., & Brownell, C. A. (2015). Individual differences in toddlers’ social understanding and prosocial behavior: Disposition or socialization? Frontiers in Psychology, 6(600), 1–11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00600
  • Harwood, M. D., & Farrar, M. J. (2006). Conflicting emotions: The connection between affective perspective taking and theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(2), 401–418. doi:10.1348/026151005X50302
  • Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children are intrinsically motivated to see others helped. Psychological Science, 23(9), 967–972. doi:10.1177/0956797612440571
  • Hipson, W. E., & Séguin, D. G. (2015). Is good fit related to good behaviour? Goodness of fit between daycare teacher–child relationships, temperament, and prosocial behaviour. Early Child Development and Care, 186(5), 785–798. doi:10.1080/03004430.2015.1061518
  • Hoff, E. (2014). Language development (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
  • Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., & Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1192–1205. doi:10.1037/dev0000140
  • LaBounty, J., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S., Lagattuta, K., & Liu, D. (2007). Mothers’ and fathers’ use of internal state talk with their young children. Review in Social Development, 17(4), 757–775. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00450.x
  • Laible, D., Carlo, G., Murphy, T., Augustine, M., & Roesch, S. (2014). Predicting children’s prosocial and co-operative behavior from their temperamental profiles: A person-centered approach. Social Development, 23(4), 734–752. doi:10.1111/sode.12072
  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732–739. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.013
  • Liverta Sempio, O., Marchetti, A., Castelli, I., Lecciso, F., & Pezzotta, C. (2005). Mentalizzazione e competenza sociale. La comprensione della falsa credenza nello sviluppo normale e patologico [Mentalizing and social competence: The false-belief understanding in typical and atypical development]. Milan, Italy: Franco Angeli.
  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., & de Rosnay, M. (2013). Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: Mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Development, 84(5), 1777–1790. doi:10.1111/cdev.12061
  • Menting, B., van Lier, P. A., & Koot, H. M. (2011). Language skills, peer rejection, and the development of externalizing behavior from kindergarten to fourth grade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(1), 72–79. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02279.x
  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x
  • Ornaghi, V., Brazzelli, E., Grazzani, I., Agliati, A., & Lucarelli, M. (2017). Does training toddlers in emotion knowledge lead to changes in their prosocial and aggressive behavior toward peers at nursery? Early Education & Development, 28(4), 396–414. doi:10.1080/10409289.2016.1238674
  • Ornaghi, V., Brockmeier, J., & Grazzani, I. (2014). Enhancing social cognition by training children in emotion understanding: A primary school study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 26–39. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.005
  • Ornaghi, V., Grazzani, I., Cherubin, E., Conte, E., & Piralli, F. (2015). “Let’s talk about emotions”: The effect of conversational training on preschoolers’ emotion comprehension and prosocial orientation. Social Development, 24(1), 166–183. doi:10.1111/sode.12091
  • Ornaghi, V., Pepe, A., & Grazzani, I. (2016). False-belief understanding and language ability mediate the relationship between emotion comprehension and prosocial orientation in preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(1534), 1–12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01534
  • Paulus, M., Kuhn-Popp, N., Licata, M., Sodian, B., & Meinhardt, J. (2013). Neural correlates of prosocial behavior in infancy: Different neurophysiological mechanisms support the emergence of helping and comforting. NeuroImage, 66, 522–530. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.041
  • Pepe, A., & Addimando, L. (2014). Teacher-parent relationships: Influence of gender and education on organizational parents’ counterproductive behaviors. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29(3), 503–519. doi:10.1007/s10212-014-0210-0
  • Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for congeneric measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21(2), 173–184. doi:10.1177/01466216970212006
  • Rhee, S. H., Boeldt, D. L., Friedman, N. P., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., Young, S. E., … Zahn-Waxler, C. (2013). The role of language in concern and disregard for others in the first years of life. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 197–214. doi:10.1037/a0028318
  • Rieffe, C., & Wiefferink, C. H. (2017). Happy faces, sad faces: Emotion understanding in toddlers and preschoolers with language impairments. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 62, 40–49. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2016.12.018
  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Devitt, K., & Crowe, E. (2006). What mothers say and what they do: The relation between parenting, theory of mind, language and conflict/cooperation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 105–124. doi:10.1348/026151005X82848
  • Sette, S., Bassett, H. H., Baumgartner, E., & Denham, S. A. (2015). Structure and validity of affect knowledge test (AKT) in a sample of Italian preschoolers. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 176(5), 330–347. doi:10.1080/00221325.2015.1075466
  • Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C. C., & Henry, J. D. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development, 86(4), 1159–1174. doi:10.1111/cdev.12372
  • Stella, G., Pizzoli, C., & Tressoldi, P. (2000). Peabody: Test di vocabolario recettivo [The Peabody: Receptive vocabulary test]. Turin, Italy: Omega.
  • Strand, P. S., Downs, A., & Barbosa-Leiker, C. (2016). Does facial expression recognition provide a toehold for the development of emotion understanding? Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1182–1191. doi:10.1037/dev0000144
  • Svetlova, M., Nichols, S. R., & Brownell, C. A. (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child Development, 81(6), 1814–1827. doi:10.1111/cdev.2010.81.issue-6
  • Thompson, R. A., & Newton, E. K. (2013). Baby altruists? Examining the complexity of prosocial motivation in young children. Infancy, 18(1), 120–133. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00139.x
  • Tisak, M. S., Holub, S. C., & Tisak, J. (2007). What nice things do boys and girls do? Preschoolers’ perspectives of peers’ behaviors at school and at home. Early Education & Development, 18(2), 183–199. doi:10.1080/10409280701282686
  • Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Parental presence and encouragement do not influence helping in young children. Infancy, 18(3), 345–368. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00120.x
  • Wellman, H. M. (1991). From desires to beliefs: Acquisition of a theory of mind. In A. Withen (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading (pp. 19–38). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
  • Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
  • Wu, Z., & Su, Y. (2014). How do preschoolers’ sharing behaviors relate to their theory of mind understanding? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 120, 73–86. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.007
  • Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). The origins of empathic concern. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 107–130. doi:10.1007/BF00991639

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.