Publication Cover
Psychological Inquiry
An International Journal for the Advancement of Psychological Theory
Volume 28, 2017 - Issue 2-3
551
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Commentaries

Separating Identity and Value in the Identity-Value Model

&

References

  • Ainslie, G. (2005). Précis of breakdown of will. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 635–50–73. doi:10.1017/S0140525×05000117
  • Baek, E. C., Scholz, C., O'Donnell, M. B., & Falk, E. B. (2017). The value of sharing information: A neural account of information transmission. Psychological Science. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0956797617695073
  • Bartels, D. M., & Urminsky, O. (2011). On intertemporal selfishness: How the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 182–198. doi:10.1086/658339
  • Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T., & Kable, J. W. (2013). The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage, 76, 412–427. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063
  • Baumeister, R. F. (2011). Self and identity: A brief overview of what they are, what they do, and how they work. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1234(1), 48–55. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06224.x
  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science (New York, N.Y.), 275(5304), 1293–1295. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9036851
  • Benoit, R. G., Gilbert, S. J., & Burgess, P. W. (2011). A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(18), 6771–6779. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6559-10.2011
  • Camille, N., Griffiths, C. A., Vo, K., Fellows, L. K., & Kable, J. W. (2011). Ventromedial frontal lobe damage disrupts value maximization in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(20), 7527–7532. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6527-10.2011
  • Clithero, J. A., & Rangel, A. (2014). Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(9), 1289–1302. doi:10.1093/scan/nst106
  • Cooper, N., Bassett, D. S., & Falk, E. B. (2017). Coherent activity between brain regions that code for value is linked to the malleability of human behavior. Scientific Reports, 7, 43250. doi:10.1038/srep43250
  • Cooper, N., Kable, J. W., Kim, B. K., & Zauberman, G. (2013). Brain activity in valuation regions while thinking about the future predicts individual discount rates. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(32), 13150–13156. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0400-13.2013
  • Cooper, N., Tompson, S., O'Donnell, M. B., & Falk, E. B. (2015). Brain activity in self- and value-related regions in response to online antismoking messages predicts behavior change. Journal of Media Psychology, 27(3), 93–109. doi:10.1027/1864-1105/a000146
  • Duckworth, A. L., Kirby, T., Gollwitzer, A., & Oettingen, G. (2013). From fantasy to action: Mental Contrasting with Implementation Intentions (MCII) improves academic performance in children. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(6), 745–753. doi:10.1177/1948550613476307
  • Ersner-Hershfield, H., Garton, M. T., Ballard, K., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Knutson, B. (2009). Don't stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences in future self-continuity account for saving. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(4), 280–286. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19774230
  • Ersner-Hershfield, H., Wimmer, G. E., & Knutson, B. (2009). Saving for the future self: Neural measures of future self-continuity predict temporal discounting. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 85–92. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn042
  • Falk, E. B., O'Donnell, M. B., Cascio, C. N., Tinney, F., Kang, Y., Lieberman, M. D., & Strecher, V. J. (2015). Self-affirmation alters the brain's response to health messages and subsequent behavior change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(7), 1977–1982. doi:10.1073/pnas.1500247112
  • Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2004). Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 15(1), 58–63. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh108
  • Fudenberg, D., & Levine, D. K. (2006). A dual-self model of impulse control. American Economic Review, 96(5), 1449–1476. doi:10.1257/aer.96.5.1449
  • Fujita, K., & Carnevale, J. J. (2012). Transcending temptation through abstraction: The role of construal level in self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4), 248–252. doi:10.1177/0963721412449169
  • Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(3), 351–367. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.orgjournals/psp/90/3/351
  • Henri-Bhargava, A., Simioni, A., & Fellows, L. K. (2012). Ventromedial frontal lobe damage disrupts the accuracy, but not the speed, of value-based preference judgments. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1536–1542. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.006
  • Hershfield, H. E. (2011). Future self-continuity: How conceptions of the future self transform intertemporal choice. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1235, 30–43. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06201.x
  • Hershfield, H. E., Goldstein, D. G., Sharpe, W. F., Fox, J., Yeykelis, L., Carstensen, L. L., & Bailenson, J. N. (2011). Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future self. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, S23–S37. doi:10.1509/jmkr.48.SPL.S23
  • Hutcherson, C. A., Bushong, B., & Rangel, A. (2015). A neurocomputational model of altruistic choice and its implications. Neuron, 87(2), 451–462. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.031
  • Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 10(12), 1625–1633. doi:10.1038/nn2007
  • Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2010). An “as soon as possible” effect in human intertemporal decision making: Behavioral evidence and neural mechanisms. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(5), 2513–2531. doi:10.1152/jn.00177.2009
  • Krajbich, I., Armel, C., & Rangel, A. (2010). Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13(10), 1292–1298. doi:10.1038/nn.2635
  • Krajbich, I., & Rangel, A. (2011). Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(33), 13852–13857. doi:10.1073/pnas.1101328108
  • Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 661–679. doi:10.1017/S0140525×12003196
  • Lempert, K. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2016). The malleability of intertemporal choice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 64–74. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.005
  • McGuire, J. T., & Kable, J. W. (2012). Decision makers calibrate behavioral persistence on the basis of time-interval experience. Cognition, 124(2), 216–226. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.008
  • McGuire, J. T., & Kable, J. W. (2013). Rational temporal predictions can underlie apparent failures to delay gratification. Psychological Review, 120(2), 395–410. doi:10.1037/a0031910
  • McGuire, J. T., & Kable, J. W. (2015). Medial prefrontal cortical activity reflects dynamic re-evaluation during voluntary persistence. Nature Neuroscience, 18(5), 760–766. doi:10.1038/nn.3994
  • Medin, D. L., & Smith, E. E. (1984). Concepts and concept formation. Annual Review of Psychology, 35(1), 113–138. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.35.020184.000553
  • Mitchell, J. P., Schirmer, J., Ames, D. L., & Gilbert, D. T. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex predicts intertemporal choice. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(4), 857–866. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21479
  • Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Murray, R. J., Schaer, M., & Debbané, M. (2012). Degrees of separation: A quantitative neuroimaging meta-analysis investigating self-specificity and shared neural activation between self- and other-reflection. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(3), 1043–1059. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.013
  • Oettingen, G. (2012). Future thought and behaviour change. European Review of Social Psychology, 23(1), 1–63. doi:10.1080/10463283.2011.643698
  • Oettingen, G., & Mayer, D. (2002). The motivating function of thinking about the future: Expectations versus fantasies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1198–1212. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12416922
  • Oettingen, G., & Wadden, T. A. (1991). Expectation, fantasy, and weight loss: Is the impact of positive thinking always positive? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15(2), 167–175. doi:10.1007/BF01173206
  • Palombo, D. J., Keane, M. M., & Verfaellie, M. (2015). The medial temporal lobes are critical for reward-based decision making under conditions that promote episodic future thinking. Hippocampus, 25(3), 345–353. doi:10.1002/hipo.22376
  • Parthasarathi, T., McConnell, M. H., Luery, J., & Kable, J. W. (2017). The vivid present: Visualization abilities are associated with steep discounting of future rewards. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 289. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00289
  • Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron, 66(1), 138–148. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.026
  • Phelps, E. A., Lempert, K. M., & Sokol-Hessner, P. (2014). Emotion and decision making: Multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 37, 263–290. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014119
  • Ross, D. (2005). Economic theory and cognitive science: Microexplanation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Ross, D. (2008). Midbrain mutiny: The picoeconomics and neuroeconomics of disordered gambling: Economic theory and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Sasse, L. K., Peters, J., Büchel, C., & Brassen, S. (2015). Effects of prospective thinking on intertemporal choice: The role of familiarity. Human Brain Mapping, 36(10), 4210–4221. doi:10.1002/hbm.22912
  • Scholz, C., Baek, E. C., O'Donnell, M. B., Kim, H. S., Cappella, J. N., & Falk, E. B. (2017). A neural model of valuation and information virality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(11), 2881–2886. doi:10.1073/pnas.1615259114

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.