Publication Cover
Psychological Inquiry
An International Journal for the Advancement of Psychological Theory
Volume 33, 2022 - Issue 4
509
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Target Article

Dynamics of Internal Attention and Internally-Directed Cognition: The Attention-to-Thoughts (A2T) Model

References

  • Alderson-Day, B., & Fernyhough, C. (2015). Inner speech: Development, cognitive functions, phenomenology, and neurobiology. Psychological Bulletin, 141(5), 931–965. doi:10.1037/bul0000021
  • Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: A generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 323–338. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.323
  • Amir, I., Ruimi, L., & Bernstein, A. (2021). Simulating thoughts to measure and study internal attention in mental health. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 2251. 10.1038/s41598-021-81756-w.
  • Amir, I., Zvielli, A., & Bernstein, A. (2016). (De)coupling of our eyes and our mind’s eye: A dynamic process perspective on attentional bias. Emotion. American Psychological Association, 16(7), 978–986. doi:10.1037/emo0000172
  • Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1316, 29–52. doi:10.1111/nyas.12360
  • Atkinson, A. L., Berry, E. D. J., Waterman, A. H., Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., & Allen, R. J. (2018). Are there multiple ways to direct attention in working memory? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1424(1), 115–126. doi:10.1111/nyas.13634
  • Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437–443. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010
  • Axelrod, V., Rees, G., & Bar, M. (2017). The default network and the combination of cognitive processes that mediate self-generated thought. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(12), 896–910. doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0244-9
  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 1–29. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
  • Bernstein, A., Hadash, Y., Lichtash, Y., Tanay, G., Shepherd, K., & Fresco, D. M. (2015). Decentering and related constructs: A critical review and metacognitive processes model. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 599–617. doi:10.1177/1745691615594577
  • Bor, D., & Seth, A. (2012). Consciousness and the Prefrontal Parietal Network: Insights from attention, working memory, and chunking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 63. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00063
  • Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21(1), 9–16. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3
  • Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H. L. J., Dalege, J., Kievit, R. A., & Haig, B. D. (2021). Theory construction methodology: A practical framework for building theories in psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 756–766. doi:10.1177/1745691620969647
  • Buetti, S., & Lleras, A. (2016). Distractibility is a function of engagement, not task difficulty: Evidence from a new oculomotor capture paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(10), 1382–1405. doi:10.1037/xge0000213
  • Buzsáki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:Oso/9780195301069.001.0001
  • Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., Olson, I. R., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(8), 613–625. doi:10.1038/nrn2459
  • Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 8719–8724. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900234106
  • Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17(11), 718–731. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.113
  • Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 73–101. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100427
  • Chun, M. M., & Johnson, M. K. (2011). Memory: Enduring traces of perceptual and reflective attention. Neuron, 72(4), 520–535. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.026
  • Coifman, K. G., Kane, M. J., Bishop, M., Matt, L. M., Nylocks, K. M., & Aurora, P. (2019). Predicting negative affect variability and spontaneous emotion regulation: Can working memory span tasks estimate emotion regulatory capacity? Emotion, 21(2), 297–314. doi:10.1037/emo0000585
  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. doi:10.1038/nrn755
  • Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Soleti, E., & Rimé, B. (2013). Negative emotional experiences arouse rumination and affect working memory capacity. Emotion, 13(5), 867–880. doi:10.1037/a0032492
  • Davey, G. C. L., Bickerstaffe, S., & MacDonald, B. A. (2006). Experienced disgust causes a negative interpretation bias: A causal role for disgust in anxious psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(10), 1375–1384. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.006
  • Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cognition & Emotion, 12(3), 307–330. doi:10.1080/026999398379628
  • Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature, 441(7095), 876–879. doi:10.1038/nature04766
  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193–222. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
  • Dixon, M. L., Fox, K. C. R., & Christoff, K. (2014). A framework for understanding the relationship between externally and internally directed cognition. Neuropsychologia, 62, 321–330. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.024
  • Dunne, J. D., Thompson, E., & Schooler, J. (2019). Mindful meta-awareness: Sustained and non-propositional. Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 307–311. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.003
  • Ehring, T., & Behar, E. (2020.) Transdiagnostic view on worrying and other negative mental content. In Generalized anxiety disorder and worrying: A comprehensive handbook for clinicians and researchers. (eds A.L. Gerlach and A.T. Gloster) Wiley-Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9781119189909.ch4
  • Ehring, T., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic process. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1(3), 192–205. doi:10.1680/ijct.2008.1.3.192
  • Ehring, T., Zetsche, U., Weidacker, K., Wahl, K., Schönfeld, S., & Ehlers, A. (2011). The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(2), 225–232. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.003
  • Eich, E. (1995). Searching for mood dependent memory. Psychological Science, 6(2), 67–75. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00309.x
  • Ellamil, M., Fox, K. C. R., Dixon, M. L., Pritchard, S., Todd, R. M., Thompson, E., & Christoff, K. (2016). Dynamics of neural recruitment surrounding the spontaneous arising of thoughts in experienced mindfulness practitioners. NeuroImage, 136, 186–196. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.034
  • Engen, H. G., & Anderson, M. C. (2018). Memory control: A fundamental mechanism of emotion regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 982–995. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.015
  • Epstein, J. M. (2008). Why model? Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 11(4), 12. Retrieved from http://jasss.soc.surrey.ac.uk/11/4/12.html
  • Epstein, R. (2000). The neural-cognitive basis of the Jamesian stream of thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 9(4), 550–575. doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0486
  • Everaert, J. (2021). Interpretation of ambiguity in depression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 41, 9–14. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.01.003
  • Everaert, J., Bernstein, A., Joormann, J., & Koster, E. H. W. (2020). Mapping dynamic interactions among cognitive biases in depression. Emotion Review, 12(2), 93–110. doi:10.1177/1754073919892069
  • Everaert, J., Duyck, W., & Koster, E. H. W. (2014). Attention, interpretation, and memory biases in subclinical depression: A proof-of-principle test of the combined cognitive biases hypothesis. Emotion, 14(2), 331–340. doi:10.1037/a0035250
  • Everaert, J., Grahek, I., & Koster, E. H. (2017). Individual differences in cognitive control over emotional material modulate cognitive biases linked to depressive symptoms. Cognition & Emotion, 31(4), 736–746. doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1144562
  • Everaert, J., Podina, I. R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2017). A comprehensive meta-analysis of interpretation biases in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 58, 33–48. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.005
  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336
  • Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2010). Computational models as aids to better reasoning in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(5), 329–335. doi:10.1177/0963721410386677
  • Fox, K. C. R., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Mills, C., Dixon, M. L., Markovic, J., Thompson, E., & Christoff, K. (2018). Affective neuroscience of self-generated thought. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1426(1), 25–51. doi:10.1111/nyas.13740
  • Fried, E. I., & Cramer, A. O. J. (2017). Moving forward: Challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 999–1020. doi:10.1177/1745691617705892
  • Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 129–135. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014
  • Gernsbacher, M. A., & Faust, M. E. (1991). The mechanism of suppression: A component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(2), 245–262. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.2.245
  • Gershman, S. J. (2016). Empirical priors for reinforcement learning models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 71, 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2016.01.006
  • Grahek, I., Everaert, J., Krebs, R. M., & Koster, E. H. W. W. (2018). Cognitive control in depression: Toward clinical models informed by cognitive neuroscience. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(4), 464–480. doi:10.1177/2167702618758969
  • Grahek, I., Schaller, M., & Tackett, J. L. (2021). Anatomy of a psychological theory: Integrating construct-validation and computational-modeling methods to advance theorizing. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 803–815. doi:10.1177/1745691620966794
  • Hadash, Y., & Bernstein, A. (2019). Behavioral assessment of mindfulness: Defining features, organizing framework, and review of emerging methods. Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 229–237. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.008
  • Halberstadt, J. B., Niedenthal, P. M., & Kushner, J. (1995). Resolution of lexical ambiguity by emotional state. Psychological Science, 6(5), 278–282. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00511.x
  • Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., & Mansell, W. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders: A transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Haslbeck, J., Ryan, O., Robinaugh, D., Waldorp, L., & Borsboom, D. (2019). Modeling psychopathology: From data models to formal theories.
  • Hayes, S., Hirsch, C., & Mathews, A. (2008). Restriction of working memory capacity during worry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(3), 712–717. doi:10.1037/a0012908
  • Hazy, T. E., Frank, M. J., & O'reilly, R. C. (2007). Towards an executive without a homunculus: Computational models of the prefrontal cortex/basal ganglia system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1485), 1601–1613. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2055
  • Heathcote, A., Brown, S. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2015). An introduction to good practices in cognitive modeling BT. In B. U. Forstmann & E.-J. Wagenmakers (Eds.) An introduction to model-based cognitive neuroscience (pp. 25–48). New York, NY: Springer New York. doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-2236-9_2
  • Hertel, P. T. (1997). On the contributions of deficent cognitive control to memory impairments in depression. Cognition & Emotion, 11(5–6), 569–583. doi:10.1080/026999397379890a
  • Hirsch, C. R., Krahé, C., Whyte, J., Loizou, S., Bridge, L., Norton, S., & Mathews, A. (2018). Interpretation training to target repetitive negative thinking in generalized anxiety disorder and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86(12), 1017–1030. doi:10.1037/ccp0000310
  • Hitchcock, C., Golden, A. M. J., Werner-Seidler, A., Kuyken, W., & Dalgleish, T. (2018). The impact of affective context on autobiographical recollection in depression. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(3), 315–324. doi:10.1177/2167702617740672
  • Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2009). The role of visual working memory (VWM) in the control of gaze during visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71(4), 936–949. doi:10.3758/APP.71.4.936
  • Hong, R. Y., & Cheung, M. W.-L. (2015). The structure of cognitive vulnerabilities to depression and anxiety: Evidence for a common core etiologic process based on a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(6), 892–912. doi:10.1177/2167702614553789
  • Huys, Q. J. M., Maia, T. V., & Frank, M. J. (2016). Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nature Neuroscience, 19(3), 404–413. doi:10.1038/nn.4238
  • Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 156–176. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.156
  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. II). New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company. doi:10.1037/11059-000
  • Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 182–192. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.182
  • Juarrero, A. (2000). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. Emergence, 2(2), 24–57. doi:10.1207/S15327000EM0202_03
  • Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. In Variation in working memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Kanske, P., Plitschka, J., & Kotz, S. A. (2011). Attentional orienting towards emotion: P2 and N400 ERP effects. Neuropsychologia, 49(11), 3121–3129. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.022
  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932. doi:10.1126/science.1192439
  • Kiyonaga, A., & Egner, T. (2013). Working memory as internal attention: Toward an integrative account of internal and external selection processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 228–242. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0359-y
  • Klinger, E. (1978). Modes of normal conscious flow. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of consciousness: Scientific investigations into the flow of human experience (pp. 225–258). Boston, MA: Springer US. doi:10.1007/978-1-4684-2466-9_9
  • Klinger, E. (2013). Goal Commitments and the content of thoughts and dreams: Basic principles. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 415. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00415
  • Klinger, E., Marchetti, I., & Koster, E. H. W. (2018). Spontaneous thought and goal pursuit: From functions such as planning to dysfunctions such as rumination. In The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and dreaming (pp. 215–247). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005
  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451–468. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.451
  • LeDoux, J. E., & Brown, R. (2017). A higher-order theory of emotional consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(10), E2016–E2025. doi:10.1073/pnas.1619316114
  • LeMoult, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Depression: A cognitive perspective. Clinical psychology Review, 69, 51–66. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2018.06.008
  • Lewis, M. D. (2005). Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 169–194. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0500004X
  • Logan, G. D. (2004). Cumulative progress in formal theories of attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1), 207–234. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141415
  • Lutz, A., Jha, A. P., Dunne, J. D., & Saron, C. D. (2015). Investigating the phenomenological matrix of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. American Psychologist, 70(7), 632–658. doi:10.1037/a0039585
  • MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 189–217. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052
  • Mansell, W., & McEvoy, P. M. (2017). A test of the core process account of psychopathology in a heterogenous clinical sample of anxiety and depression: A case of the blind men and the elephant? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 46, 4–10. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.06.008
  • Marchetti, I., Loeys, T., Alloy, L. B., & Koster, E. H. W. (2016). Unveiling the structure of cognitive vulnerability for depression: Specificity and overlap. PLOS One, 11(12), e0168612. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0168612
  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 188–197. doi:10.1037/a0018298
  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Drifting from slow to “D’oh!”: Working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 525–549. doi:10.1037/a0025896
  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2013). Dispatching the wandering mind? Toward a laboratory method for cuing “spontaneous” off-task thought. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 570. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00570
  • Mrkva, K., Westfall, J., & Van Boven, L. (2019). Attention drives emotion: Voluntary visual attention increases perceived emotional intensity. Psychological Science, 30(6), 942–954. doi:10.1177/0956797619844231
  • Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2019). A problem in theory. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(3), 221–229. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0522-1
  • Myers, N. E., Stokes, M. G., & Nobre, A. C. (2017). Prioritizing information during working memory: Beyond sustained internal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 449–461. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.010
  • Nobre, A. C., Coull, J. T., Maquet, P., Frith, C. D., Vandenberghe, R., & Mesulam, M. M. (2004). Orienting attention to locations in perceptual versus mental representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 363–373. doi:10.1162/089892904322926700
  • Nobre, A. C., & Stokes, M. G. (2019). Premembering experience: A hierarchy of time-scales for proactive attention. Neuron, 104(1), 132–146. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2019.08.030
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 115–121. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
  • Oberauer, K. (2009). Design for a working memory. In The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 51, pp. 45–100). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(09)51002-X
  • Oberauer, K., & Hein, L. (2012). Attention to Information in Working Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(3), 164–169. doi:10.1177/0963721412444727
  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466–478. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
  • Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158. doi:10.1038/nrn2317
  • Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., & Miles, E. (2013). Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1412–1421. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.09.012
  • Posner, M. (2016). Orienting of attention: Then and now. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(10), 1864–1875. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.937446
  • Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 91(16), 7398–7403. doi:10.1073/pnas.91.16.7398
  • Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676–682. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676
  • Ruby, F. J. M., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., & Singer, T. (2013). How self-generated thought shapes mood—The relation between mind-wandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of thoughts. PLOS One, 8(10), e77554. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077554
  • Ruimi, L., Hadash, Y., Zvielli, A., Amir, I., Goldstein, P., & Bernstein, A. (2018). Meta-awareness of dysregulated emotional attention. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(5), 658–670. doi:10.1177/2167702618776948
  • Scher, C. D., Ingram, R. E., & Segal, Z. V. (2005). Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(4), 487–510. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.01.005
  • Schwartz, J. E., & Stone, A. A. (2007). The analysis of real-time momentary data: A practical guide. In The science of real-time data capture: Self-reports in health research (pp. 76–113). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Schweizer, S., & Dalgleish, T. (2016). The impact of affective contexts on working memory capacity in healthy populations and in individuals with PTSD. Emotion, 16(1), 16–23. doi:10.1037/emo0000072
  • Seli, P., Konishi, M., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2018). The role of task difficulty in theoretical accounts of mind wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 65, 255–262. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2018.08.005
  • Shadlen, M. N., & Newsome, W. T. (1994). Noise, neural codes and cortical organization. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4(4), 569–579. doi:10.1016/0959-4388(94)90059-0
  • Sheppes, G., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11(1), 379–405. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739
  • Siemer, M. (2005). Mood-congruent cognitions constitute mood experience. Emotion, 5(3), 296–308. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.296
  • Smallwood, J. (2013a). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: A process–occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. Psychological Bulletin, 139(3), 519–535. doi:10.1037/a0030010
  • Smallwood, J. (2013b). Searching for the elements of thought: Reply to Franklin, Mrazek, Broadway, and Schooler (2013). Psychological Bulletin, 139(3), 542–547. doi:10.1037/a0031019
  • Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 441. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441
  • Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9(2), 271–276. doi:10.1037/a0014855
  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. SSRN, 66, 487–518. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331
  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946
  • Smith, P. L., & Sewell, D. K. (2013). A competitive interaction theory of attentional selection and decision making in brief, multielement displays. Psychological Review, 120(3), 589–627. doi:10.1037/a0033140
  • Teasdale, J. D., & Barnard, P. J. (1993). Affect, cognition, and change: Re-modelling depressive thought. In Affect, cognition, and change: Re-modelling depressive thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Memory & Cognition, 23(5), 551–559. doi:10.3758/BF03197257
  • Theeuwes, J. (2019). Goal-driven, stimulus-driven, and history-driven selection. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 97–101. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.024
  • Thurner, S., Hanel, R., & Klimek, P. (2018). Introduction to the theory of complex systems. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Treisman, A. (1964). Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in selective attention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3(6), 449–459. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(64)80015-3
  • Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 16(1), 55–61. doi:10.1038/nrn3857
  • van Rooij, I., & Baggio, G. (2021). Theory before the test: How to build high-verisimilitude explanatory theories in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 682–697. doi:10.1177/1745691620970604
  • van Schie, K., & Anderson, M. C. (2017). Successfully controlling intrusive memories is harder when control must be sustained. Memory, 25(9), 1201–1216. doi:10.1080/09658211.2017.1282518
  • van Vugt, M., & van der Velde, M. (2018). How does rumination impact cognition? A first mechanistic model. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(1), 175–191. doi:10.1111/tops.12318
  • Verschooren, S., Schindler, S., De Raedt, R., & Pourtois, G. (2019). Switching attention from internal to external information processing: A review of the literature and empirical support of the resource sharing account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 468–490. doi:10.3758/s13423-019-01568-y
  • Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J., & Dolan, R. (2003). Reciprocal links between emotion and attention. Human Brain Function, 2, 419–444.
  • Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163–206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163
  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological review, 101(1), 34–52. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.101.1.34
  • Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1093–1104. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.65.6.1093
  • Welhaf, M. S., Smeekens, B. A., Gazzia, N. C., Perkins, J. B., Silvia, P. J., Meier, M. E., … Kane, M. J. (2020). An exploratory analysis of individual differences in mind wandering content and consistency. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7(2), 103–125. doi:10.1037/cns0000180
  • Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1036–1061. doi:10.1037/a0030923
  • Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Herman, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., & Dalgleish, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 122–148. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122
  • Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
  • Yantis, S., & Johnston, J. C. (1990). On the locus of visual selection: Evidence from focused attention tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 135–149. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.135
  • Zetsche, U., Bürkner, P. C., & Schulze, L. (2018). Shedding light on the association between repetitive negative thinking and deficits in cognitive control–a meta-analysis. Clinical psychology Review, 63, 56–65. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2018.06.001
  • Ziegler, D. A., Janowich, J. R., & Gazzaley, A. (2018). Differential impact of interference on internally- and externally-directed attention. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1–10. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-20498-8
  • Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., & Koster, E. H. W. (2015). Temporal dynamics of attentional bias. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(5), 772–788. doi:10.1177/2167702614551572

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.