854
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Mentalizing: What’s language got to do with it?

Pages 255-275 | Received 14 Sep 2018, Accepted 25 Mar 2020, Published online: 31 May 2020

References

  • Andrés-Roqueta, C., J.E. Adriàn, R.A. Clemente, and N. Katsos. 2013. “Which are the Best Predictors of Theory of Mind Delay in Children with Specific Language Impairment?” International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 48 (6):726–37. doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12045.
  • Astington, J. W., and M. J. Edward. 2010. “Language Matters for Theory of Mind.” International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development Newsletter 45 (1):7–9.
  • Astington, J.W. 1993. The Child’s Discovery of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Astington, J.W., and J.M. Jenkins. 1999. “A Longitudinal Study of the Relation between Language and Theory-of-mind Development.” Developmental Psychology 35 (5):1311–20. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311.
  • Astington, J.W., and L. A. Dack. 2008. “Theory of Mind.” In Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development, edited by MM Haith and JB Benson, Vol. 3, 343–56. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S. 2000. “Theory of Mind and Autism: A Fifteen Year Review.” In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neurosciences, edited by S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg and D.J. Cohen, 2nd ed., 3–20. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., A. Leslie, and U. Frith. 1985. “Does the Autistic Child Have a “Theory of Mind”?” Cognition 21 (1):37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M., & Frith, U. 1986. Mechanical, behavioural and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(2),113–125.
  • Bishop, V. M. 2014. Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Bloom, L., M. Rispoli, B. Gartner, and J. Hafitz. 1989. “Acquisition of Complementation.” Journal of Child Language 16 (1):101–20. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013465.
  • Boroditsky, L. 2001. “Does Langage Shape Thought ?: Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conception of Time.” Cognitive Psychology 43 (1):1–22. Consulté sur http://www.idealibrary.com.
  • Bowerman, B., and S. Levinson. 2001. “Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development.” Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics doi:10.1017/CBO9780511620669.
  • Carlson, S.M., L. J. Moses, and H.R. Hix. 1998. “The Role of Inhibitory Processes in Young Children’s Difficulties with Deception and False Belief.” Child Development 69 (3):672–91. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.00672.x.
  • Chen, S.H., and V. Bernard-Opitz. 1993. “Comparison of Personal and Computer-assisted Instruction for Children with Autism.” Mental Retardation 31 (6):368–76.
  • Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. 2007. Do Children with Autism have a Theory of Mind?. A non-verbal test of autism vs. specific language impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 716–723.
  • Craven, P.L. 2005. Testing Expression Effects of Language and Inhibition within a Series of False Belief Tasks. Doctoral dissertation, The Pennsylvania State University, U.S.A.
  • de Villiers, J. 1995 April. Steps in the Mastery of Sentence Complements Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Indianapolis, IN.
  • de Villiers, J. 2000. “Language and Theory of Mind. What are the Developmental Relationships?” In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, edited by S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg and D.J. Cohen, 83–123. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • de Villiers, J. 2005. “Can Language Acquisition Give Children a Point of View.” In Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind, edited by J.W. Astington and J.A, 186–219. Baird. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • de Villiers, J. 2007. “The Interface of Language and Theory of Mind.” Lingua 117 (11):1858–78. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2006.11.006.
  • de Villiers, J., and J. Pyers. 1997. “Complementing Cognition: The Relationship between Language and Theory of Mind.” Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 136–47. Boston, Massachusetts.
  • de Villiers, J., and P. de Villiers. 2000. “Linguistic Determination and the Understanding of False Beliefs.” In Children’s Reasoning and the Mind, edited by P. Mitchell and K. J. Riggs, 191–228. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • de Villiers, J. G., and P. A. de Villiers. 2009. “Complements Enable Representation of the Contents of False Beliefs: The Evolution of a Theory.” In Language Acquisition, edited by S. Foster-Cohen,  169–195. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave-McMillan Press.
  • de Villiers, J.G., and J. Pyers. 2002. “Complements to Cognition: A Longitudinal Study of the Relationship between Complex Syntax and False Belief Understanding.” Cognitive Development 17 (1):1037–60. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4.
  • de Villiers, P.A., F. Burns, and B.Z. Pearson. 2003. “The Role of Language in the Theory of Mind Development of Language-Impaired Children: Complementing Theories.” In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, edited by B. Beachley, A. Brown and F. Conlin, 232–42. Boston, Massachusetts, U.S.A: Cascadilla Press.
  • Dennett, D. 1978. “Beliefs about Beliefs.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (4):568–70. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076664.
  • Derksen, D.G., M.C. Hunsche, M. E. Giroux, D.A. Connolly, and D.M. Bernstein. 2018. “A Systematic Review of Theory of Mind’s Precursors and Functions.” Zeitschrift für Psychologie 226 (2):87–97. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000325.
  • Diamond, A., & Kirkham, N. 2005. Not quite as grown-up as we like to think: Parallels between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychological Science, 16(4), 291–297.
  • Diessel, H. 2004. The Acquisition of Complex Sentences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Dunn, L. M., C. M. Thériault-Whalen, and L. M. Dunn. 1993. Échelle De Vocabulaire En Images Peabody. Toronto: Psychan.
  • Durrleman, S., J. Da Costa, and H. Delage. 2016. Différencier l’Idée de la Réalité par Exercices (DIRE). Geneva, Switzerland: iPad application, University of Geneva.
  • Durrleman, S., and J. Franck. 2015. “Exploring Links between Language and Cognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Complement Sentences, False Belief, and Executive Functioning.” Journal of Communication Disorders 54:15–31. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.12.001.
  • Durrleman, S., M. Burnel, and A. Reboul. 2017. “Theory of Mind in SLI Revisited: Links with Syntax, Comparisons with ASD.” International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 52 (6):816–830.
  • Durrleman, S., M. Burnel, E. Thommen, N. Foudon, S. Sonié, A. Reboul, and P. Fourneret. 2016. “The Language-cognition Interface in ASD: Complement Sentences and False Belief Reasoning.” Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 21:109–20. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2015.10.003.
  • Durrleman, S., P. Gatignol, and H. Delage. 2017. “‘Can theory of mind be improved thanks to grammatical training? A study of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Specific Language Impairment (‘La théorie de l’esprit peut-elle s’améliorer grâce à un entraînement grammatical? Une étude chez les enfants atteints de troubles du spectre autistique et de troubles spécifiques du langage’).” In Actes du XVII° Congrès de L’Union Nationale pour le Développement de la Recherche et de l’Evaluation en Orthophonie (UNADREO), 181–201. Paris.  Isbergues, France: Prtho Édition. ISBN is 978-2-36235-100-6.
  • Durrleman, S., and S. Zufferey. 2009. “The Nature of Syntactic Impairment in Autism.” Rivista di Grammatica Generativa 34:57–86.
  • Fisher, N., F. Happé, and J. Dunn. 2005. “The Relationship between Vocabulary, Grammar, and False Belief Task Performance in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders and Children with Moderate Learning Difficulties.” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 46 (4):409–19. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00371.x.
  • Frye, D., P. D. Zelazo, and J. A. Burack. 1998. “I. Cognitive Complexity and Control: Implications for Theory of Mind in Typical and Atypical Development.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 7 (4):116–21. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10774754.
  • Gentner, D., and S. Goldin-Meadow. 2003. Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Gopnik, A., and J. W. Astington. 1988. “Children’s Understanding of Representational and Its Relation to the Understanding of False Belief and the Appearance-reality Distinction.” Child Development 59 (1):26–37. doi:10.2307/1130386.
  • Hale, C. M., and H. Tager‐Flusberg. 2003. “The Influence of Language on Theory of Mind: A Training Study.” Developmental Science 6 (3):346–59. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00289.
  • Happé, F. G. E. 1995. “The Role of Age and Verbal Ability in the Theory of Mind Task Performance of Subjects with Autism.” Child Development 66 (3):843–55. doi:10.2307/1131954.
  • Helloin, M.-C., and M.-P. Thibault. 2006. l’EXALANG 3-6. Mont-Saint: Ortho-Mothus.
  • Hinzen, W. 2017. “Reference across Pathologies: A New Linguistic Lens on Disorders of Thought. Target Article with Responses and A Reply.” Theoretical Linguistics 43 (3–4):169–232. doi:10.1515/tl-2017-0013.
  • Holmes, A. M. 2002. “Theory of Mind and Behavior Disorders in Children with Specific Language Impairment  (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI No. AAINQ64711)
  • Hughes, C., and J. Dunn. 1998. “Understanding Mind and Emotion: Longitudinal Associations with Mental-state Talk between Young Friends.” Developmental Psychology 34 (5):1026–37. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.1026.
  • Huttinger, P.L. 1996. “Computer Applications in Programs for Young Children with Disabilities: Recurring Themes.” Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities 11 (2):105–14. doi:10.1177/108835769601100206.
  • Jenkins, J. M., and J. W. Astington. 1996. “Cognitive Factors and Family Structure Associated with Theory of Mind Development in Young Children.” Developmental Psychology 32 (1):70–78. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.1.70.
  • Jester, M., and C. J. Johnson. 2016. “Differences in Theory of Mind and Pretend Play Associations in Children with and without Specific Language Impairment.” Infant Child Dev. 25:24–42.
  • Khomsi, A., J. Khomsi, F. Pasquet, and A. Parbeau-Gueno. 2007. BILO, Bilan Informatisé Du Langage Oral.  (BILO 2 ; BILO 3C). ECPA, Paris.
  • King, A. M., M. Thomeczek, and G. Voreis. 2014. “iPad Use in Children and Young Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Observational Study.” Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30 (2):159–73. doi:10.1177/0265659013510922.
  • Lind, S. E., and D. M. Bowler. 2009. “Language and Theory of Mind in Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Relationship between Complement Syntax and False Belief Task Performance.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39 (6):929–37. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0702-y.
  • Lohmann, H., and M. Tomasello. 2003. “The Role of Language in the Development of False Belief Understanding: A Training Study.” Child Development 74 (4):1130–44. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00597.
  • Loukusa, S., L. Mäakinen, S. Kuusikko-Gauffin, H. Ebeling, and I. Moilanen. 2014. “Theory of Mind and Emotion Recognition Skills in Children with Specific Language Impairment, Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development: Group Differences and Connection to Knowledge of Grammatical Morphology, Word-finding Abilities and Verbal Working Memory.” International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 49 (4):498–507. doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12091.
  • Maggio, V., N. E. Granana, A. Richaudeau, S. Torres, A. Giannotti, and A. M. Suburo. 2014. “Behavior Problems in Children with Specific Language Impairment.” Journal of Child Neurology 29 (2):194–202. doi:10.1177/0883073813509886.
  • McCabe, P. C. 2005. “Social and Behavioral Correlates of Preschoolers with Specific Language Impairment.” Psychology in the Schools 42 (4):373–87. doi:10.1002/pits.20064.
  • McCabe, P. C., and D. J. Marshall. 2006. “Measuring the Social Competence of Preschool Children with Specific Language Impairment: Correspondence among Informant Rating and Behavioral Observations.” Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 26 (4):234–46. doi:10.1177/02711214060260040401.
  • Miller, C. A. 2004. “False Belief and Sentence Complement Performance in Children with Specific Language Impairment.” International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 39 (2):191–213. doi:10.1080/13682820310001616994.
  • Milligan, K., J. W. Astington, and L. A. Dack. 2007. “Language and Theory of Mind: Meta‐analysis of the Relation between Language Ability and False‐belief Understanding.” Child Development 78 (2):622–46. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x.
  • Murdock LC, Ganz J, Crittendon J. 2013. Use of an iPad play story to increase play dialogue of preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43 (9), 2174–2189 9 43 doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1770-6
  • O’Malley, P., Lewis, M. E. B., Donehower, C., & Stone, D. 2014. Effectiveness of using iPads to increase academic task completion by students with autism. Universal Journal of Educational Research, 2(1), pp. 90-97.
  • Onishi, K. H., and R. Baillargeon. 2005. “Do 15-month-old Infants Understand False Beliefs?” Science 308 (5719):255–58. doi:10.1126/science.1107621.
  • Perner, J., and T. Ruffman. 2005. “Infants’ Insight into the Mind: How Deep?” Science 308 (5719):214–16. doi:10.1126/science.1111656.
  • Povinelli, D., and J. Vonk. 2004. “We Don’t Need a Microscope to Explore the Chimpanzee’s Mind.” Mind and Language 19 (1):1–28. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.2004.00244.x.
  • Prior, M., B. Dahlstrom, and T. L. Squires. 1990. “Autism Children’s Knowledge of Thinking and Feeling States in Other People.” Journal of Child and Psychology and Psychatry, and Allied Disciplines 31 (4):587–601. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00799.x.
  • Raven, J., J. C. Raven, and J. H. Court. 1998. Raven Manual: Section 4, Advanced Progressive Matrices. 1998 ed. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press Ltd.
  • Roid, G. H., and L. J. Miller. 1997. Leiter International Performance Scale-revised: Examiners Manual. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting.
  • Ruffman, T., L. Slade, K. Rowlandson, C. Rumsey, and A. Garnham. 2003. “How Language Relates to Belief, Desire, and Emotion Understanding.” Cognitive Development 18 (2):139–58. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00002-9.
  • Schick, B., P. De Villiers, J. De Villiers, and R. Hoffmeister. 2007. “Language and Theory of Mind: A Study of Deaf Children.” Child Development 78 (2):376–96. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x.
  • Senju, A., V. Southgate, Y. Miura, T. Matsui, T. Hasegawa, and Y. Tojo. 2010. “Absence of Spontaneous Action Anticipation by False Belief Attribution in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder.” Development and Psychopathology 22 (2):353–60. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000106.
  • Sirois, S., and I. Jackson. 2007. “Social Cognition in Infancy: A Critical Review of Research on Higher Order Abilities.” European Journal of Developmental Psychology 4 (1):46–64. doi:10.1080/17405620601047053.
  • Southgate, V., A. Senju, and G. Csibra. 2007. “Action Anticipation through Attribution of False Belief by 2-year-olds.” Psychological Science 18 (7):587–92. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x.
  • Steel, G., M. Rose, and P. Eadie. 2016. “The Production of Complement Clauses in Children with Language Impairment.” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 59 (2):330–41. doi:10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-15-0001.
  • Tager-Flusberg, H., and R. M. Joseph. 2005. “How Language Facilitates the Acquisition of False-Belief Understanding in Children with Autism.” In Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind, edited by J. W. Astington and J. A. Baird, 298–318. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Tuller, L., C. Henry, E. Sizaret, and M. A. Barthez. 2012. “Specific Language Impairment at Adolescence: Avoiding Complexity.” Applied Psycholinguistics 33 (1):161–84. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000312.
  • Vissders, C., and S. Koolen. 2016. “Theory of Mind Deficits and Social Emotion Fuunctioning in Preschoolers with Specific Language Impairment.” Frontiers in Psychology 7:1734.
  • Wellman, H., D. Cross, and J. Watson. 2001. “Meta-analysis of Theory of Mind Development: The Truth about False Belief.” Child Development 72 (3):655–84. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304.
  • Wellman, H. M, and D. Liu. 2004. “Scaling of Theory-of-mind Task.” Child Development 75 (2):523–41. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x.
  • Wilson, M. S., and B. J. Fox. 2013. Language for Theory of Mind: Understanding Others’ Perceptions, Wants, and Needs. Winooski, VT: Laureate Learning Systems.
  • Wimmer, H., and J. Perner. 1983. “Beliefs about Beliefs: Representation and Constraining Function of Wrong Beliefs in Young Children’s Understanding of Deception.” Cognition 13 (1):103–28. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5.
  • Woolfe, T., S.C. Want, and M. Siegal. 2002. “Signposts to Development: Theory of Mind in Deaf Children.” Child Development 73 (3):768–78. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00437.
  • Zelazo, P. D. 2006. “The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A Method of Assessing Executive Function in Children.” Nature Protocols 1 (1):297–301. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006.46.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.