515
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The Effects of COVID-19 Infection on Opposition to COVID-19 Policies: Evidence from the U.S. Congress

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

References

  • Abadie, A., Diamond, A., & Hainmueller, J. (2010). Synthetic control methods for comparative case studies: Estimating the effect of California’s tobacco control program. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 105(490), 493–505. https://doi.org/10.1198/jasa.2009.ap08746
  • Amat, F., Arenas, A., Falcó-Gimeno, A., & Muñoz, J. (2020). Pandemics meet democracy. Experimental evidence from the covid-19 crisis in Spain. SocArXiv papers, 6, 1–31.
  • Andreottola, G. (2021). Flip-flopping and electoral concerns. The Journal of Politics, 83(4), 1669–1680. https://doi.org/10.1086/715070
  • Arceneaux, K., Dunaway, J., Soroka, S., & Wilson, R. K. (2018). Elites are people, too: The effects of threat sensitivity on policymakers’ spending priorities. PLOS one, (4), e0193781. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193781
  • Asimakopoulou, K., Hoorens, V., Speed, E., Coulson, N. S., Antoniszczak, D., Collyer, F., Deschrijver, E., Dubbin, L., Faulks, D., Forsyth, R., Goltsi, V., Harsløf, I., Larsen, K., Manaras, I., Olczak‐Kowalczyk, D., Willis, K., Xenou, T., & Scambler, S. (2020). Comparative optimism about infection and recovery from Covid-19; implications for adherence with lockdown advice. Health Expectations, 23(6), 1502–1511. https://doi.org/10.1111/hex.13134
  • Athey, S., Bayati, M., Doudchenko, N., Imbens, G., & Khosravi, K. (2021). Matrix completion methods for causal panel data models. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 116(536), 1716–1730. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.2021.1891924
  • Attema, A. E., L’haridon, O., Raude, J., Seror, V., & Group, C. (2021). Beliefs and risk perceptions about covid-19: Evidence from two successive French representative surveys during lockdown. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 619145. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.619145
  • Baccini, L., & Leemann, L. (2021). Do natural disasters help the environment? How voters respond and what that means. Political Science Research and Methods, 9(3), 468–484. https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2020.25
  • Bai, J. (2009). Panel data models with interactive fixed effects. Econometrica, 77(4), 1229–1279.
  • Baker, A. C., Larcker, D. F., & Wang, C. C. (2022). How much should we trust staggered difference-in-differences estimates? Journal of Financial Economics, 144(2), 370–395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfineco.2022.01.004
  • Bayerlein, M., Boese, V.A, Gates, S., Kamin, K., & Mansoob Murshed, S.M. (2021). Populism and covid-19: How populist governments (Mis) handle the pandemic. Journal of Political Institutions and Political Economy, 2(3), 389–428. https://doi.org/10.1561/113.00000043
  • Bayes, R., & Druckman, J. N. (2021). Motivated reasoning and climate change. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 27–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.009
  • Bisbee, J., & Da in Lee, D. (2022). Objective facts and elite cues: Partisan responses to covid-19. The Journal of Politics, 84(3), 1278–1291. https://doi.org/10.1086/716969
  • Bisgaard, M., & Slothuus, R. (2018). Partisan elites as culprits? How party cues shape partisan perceptual gaps. American Journal of Political Science, 62(2), 456–469. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12349
  • Biswas, M., Rahaman, S., Kumar Biswas, T., Haque, Z., & Ibrahim, B. (2021). Association of sex, age, and comorbidities with mortality in covid-19 patients: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Intervirology, 64(1), 36–47. https://doi.org/10.1159/000512592
  • Bol, D., Giani, M., Blais, A., & John Loewen, P. (2021). The effect of COVID-19 lockdowns on political support: Some good news for democracy? European Journal of Political Research, 60(2), 497–505. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.12401
  • Brantly, C., & Sant’anna, P. H. (2021). Difference-in-differences with multiple time periods. Journal of Econometrics, 225(2), 200–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeconom.2020.12.001
  • Bresnahan, J., & Caygle, H. (2020). Pelosi announces big expansion of COVID testing for lawmakers. Politico. November 2020. https://www.politico.com/news/2020/11/05/pelosi-covid-testing-congress-434399
  • Broockman, D. E., & Skovron, C. (2018). Bias in perceptions of public opinion among political elites. American Political Science Review, 112(3), 542–563. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055418000011
  • Bruine de Bruin, W., & Isaacowitz, D. (2021). Age differences in COVID-19 risk perceptions and mental health: Evidence from a National US survey conducted in March 2020. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 76(2), e24–29. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa074
  • Card, D., & Krueger, A. B. (1993). Minimum wages and employment: A case study of the fast food industry in new jersey and Pennsylvania. National bureau of economic research working paper 4509.
  • Cillizza, C. (2021). Two charts that show just how old this congress actually is. CNNPolitics January 2021. https://edition.cnn.com/2021/01/29/politics/congress-age/index.html
  • Cook, N., & Choi, M. (2020). Trump rallies his base to treat coronavirus as a ‘Hoax’. Politico. 2020. https://www.politico.eu/article/trump-rallies-his-base-to-treat-coronavirus-as-a-hoax/
  • Cormack, L., & Meidlinger, K. (2021). Congressional communication in a pandemic: ‘Follow the leader’ politics and responsive representation. Congress & the Presidency, 49(3), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/07343469.2021.1955037
  • Cunningham, S. (2021). Causal inference: The mixtape. Yale University Press.
  • Debus, M., & Tosun, J. (2021). Political ideology and vaccination willingness: Implications for policy design. Policy Sciences, 54(3), 477–491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11077-021-09428-0
  • Devlin, J., Chang, M.-W., Lee, K., & Toutanova, K. (2018). Bert: Pre-training of deep bidirectional transformers for language understanding. Arxiv Preprint Arxiv:1810.04805.
  • De Vries, C. E., Bakker, B. N., Hobolt, S. B., & Arceneaux, K. (2021). Crisis signaling: How Italy’s coronavirus lockdown affected incumbent support in other European Countries. Political Science Research and Methods, 9(3), 451–467. https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2021.6
  • Doherty, D., Dowling, C. M., & Miller, M. G. (2016). When is changing policy positions costly for politicians? Experimental evidence. Political Behavior, 38(2), 455–484. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-015-9321-9
  • Drummond, C., & Fischhoff, B. (2017). Individuals with greater science literacy and education have more polarized beliefs on controversial science topics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(36), 9587–9592. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704882114
  • Eberl, J.-M., A Huber, R., & Greussing, E. (2021). From populism to the ‘Plandemic’: Why populists believe in covid-19 conspiracies. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 31(sup1), 272–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/17457289.2021.1924730
  • Engel-Rebitzer, E., Stokes, D. C., Meisel, Z. F., Purtle, J., Doyle, R., & Buttenheim, A. M. (2022). Partisan differences in legislators’ discussion of vaccination on twitter during the COVID-19 era: Natural language processing analysis. Jmir Infodemiology, 2(1), e32372. https://doi.org/10.2196/32372
  • Esaiasson, P., Sohlberg, J., Ghersetti, M., & Johansson, B. (2021). How the coronavirus crisis affects citizen trust in institutions and in unknown others: Evidence from ‘the Swedish experiment’. European Journal of Political Research, 60(3), 748–760. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.12419
  • Evans, J. H., & Hargittai, E. (2020). Who doesn’t trust Fauci? The public’s belief in the expertise and shared values of scientists in the covid-19 pandemic. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World, 6, 2378023120947337. https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023120947337
  • Federal Reserve Bank. (2022). Unemployment rate (UNRATE) FRED St. Louis Fed. St. Louis Fed: Economic data. August 2022. https://fred.stlouisfed.org/series/UNRATE
  • Fetzer, T., Witte, M., Hensel, L., Jachimowicz, J. M., Haushofer, J., Ivchenko, A., Caria, S., Reutskaja, E., Roth, C., Fiorin, S. & Gomez, M. (2020). Global behaviors and perceptions in the covid-19 pandemic. National Bureau of Economic Research. http://www.nber.org/papers/w27082
  • Gadarian, S. K., Wallace Goodman, S., Pepinsky, T. B., & Lupu, N. (2021). Partisanship, health behavior, and policy attitudes in the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Public Library of Science ONE, 16(4), e0249596. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249596
  • Gilardi, F., Alizadeh, M., & Kubli, M. (2023). Chatgpt outperforms crowd-workers for text-annotation tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(30). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2305016120 arXiv Preprint arXiv:2303.15056.
  • Gobillon, L., & Magnac, T. (2016). Regional policy evaluation: Interactive fixed effects and synthetic controls. Review of Economics and Statistics, 98(3), 535–551. https://doi.org/10.1162/REST_a_00537
  • Gollust, S. E., Nagler, R. H., & Franklin Fowler, E. (2020). The emergence of COVID-19 in the us: A public health and political communication crisis. Journal of Health Politics, Policy and Law, 45(6), 967–981. https://doi.org/10.1215/03616878-8641506
  • GovTrack. (2022). COVID-19 in congress. GovTrack. 2022. https://www.govtrack.us/covid-19
  • Hart, P. S., & Nisbet, E. C. (2012). Boomerang effects in science communication: How motivated reasoning and identity cues amplify opinion polarization about climate mitigation policies. Communication Research, 39(6), 701–723. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650211416646
  • Hersh, E. D. (2013). Long-term effect of September 11 on the political behavior of victims’ families and neighbors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(52), 20959–20963. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1315043110
  • Hertel-Fernandez, A., Stokes, L. C., & Stokes, L. C. (2019). Legislative staff and representation in congress. American Political Science Review, 113(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055418000606
  • Jones, B. D. (2001). Politics and the architecture of choice: Bounded rationality and governance. University of Chicago Press.
  • Kalla, J. L., & Porter, E. (2021). Correcting bias in perceptions of public opinion among American elected officials: Results from two field experiments. British Journal of Political Science, 51(4), 1792–1800. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123419000711
  • Kavakli, K. C. (2020). Did populist leaders respond to the covid-19 pandemic more slowly? Evidence from a global sample. Unpublished Manuscript. Milano: Covid crisis Lab, Bocconi University
  • Kertzer, J. D. (2022). Re-assessing elite-public gaps in political behavior. American Journal of Political Science, 66(3), 539–553. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12583
  • Kousser, T., & Tranter, B. (2018). The influence of political leaders on climate change attitudes. Global Environmental Change, 50, 100–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2018.03.005
  • Kreps, S. E., & Kriner, D. L. (2020). Model uncertainty, political contestation, and public trust in science: Evidence from the COVID-19 pandemic. Science Advances, 6(43), eabd4563. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abd4563
  • Lasco, G. (2020). Medical populism and the COVID-19 pandemic. Global Public Health, 15(10), 1417–1429. https://doi.org/10.1080/17441692.2020.1807581
  • Latkin, C. A., Dayton, L., Moran, M., Strickland, J. C., & Collins, K. (2021). Behavioral and psychosocial factors associated with covid-19 skepticism in the United States. Current Psychology, 41(11), 7918–7926. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-01211-3
  • LeBlanc, P. (2020). Republican lawmakers who downplayed coronavirus concerns face backlash over early vaccinations. CNN. CNNPolitics. 2020. Retrieved: January 6, 2022, from. https://edition.cnn.com/2020/12/21/politics/vaccine-lawmakers-backlash/index.html
  • Liu, L., Wang, Y., & Xu, Y. (2022). A practical guide to counterfactual estimators for causal inference with time-series cross-sectional data. American Journal of Political Science, 68(1), 160–176. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12723
  • MarketWatch. (2022). DJIA: Dow jones industrial average overview. Dow Jones Industrial Average. August. 2022. https://www.marketwatch.com/investing/index/djia
  • Merkley, E., & Stecula, D. A. (2021). Party cues in the news: Democratic elites, republican Backlash, and the dynamics of climate skepticism. British Journal of Political Science, 51(4), 1439–1456. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123420000113
  • Mizrahi, B., Shilo, S., Rossman, H., Kalkstein, N., Marcus, K., Barer, Y., Keshet, A., Shamir-Stein, N., Shalev, V., Zohar, A. E., Chodick, G., & Segal, E. (2020). Longitudinal symptom dynamics of COVID-19 infection. Nature Communications, 11(1), 6208. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-20053-y
  • Newman, B. J., & Hartman, T. K. (2019). Mass shootings and public support for gun control. British Journal of Political Science, 49(4), 1527–1553. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123417000333
  • Newman, D., Lewandowsky, S., & Mayo, R. (2022). Believing in nothing and believing in everything: The underlying cognitive paradox of anti-covid-19 vaccine attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 189, 111522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111522
  • Nguyen, D. Q., Vu, T., & Tuan Nguyen, A. (2020). Bertweet: A pre-trained language model for English tweets. Proceedings of the 2020 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing: System Demonstrations (pp. 9–14). Association for Computational Linguistics, Online. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/2020.emnlp-demos.2
  • NPR. (2022). State of the Union attendees needed a COVID test. 6 legislators tested positive. Npr. March. 2022. https://www.npr.org/2022/03/01/1083845000/state-of-the-union-covid
  • Olmastroni, F., Guidi, M., Martini, S., & Isernia, P. (2021). Framing effects on the COVID-19 see-saw. Swiss Political Science Review, 27(2), 257–270. https://doi.org/10.1111/spsr.12453
  • OpenAI. (2022). GPT-3.5 language model. 2022. https://platform.openai.com/models/gpt-3.5
  • Paz, C. (2020). All the president’s lies about the coronavirus. The Atlantic. 2020. https://www.theatlantic.com/politics/archive/2020/11/trumps-lies-about-coronavirus/608647/
  • Pearce, L., & Cooper, J. (2021). Fostering COVID-19 safe behaviors using cognitive dissonance. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 43(5), 267–282. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2021.1953497
  • Pennycook, G., McPhetres, J., Bago, B., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Predictors of attitudes and misperceptions about covid-19 in Canada, the UK, and the USA. PsyArXiv Preprints, 10, 1–25.
  • Pereira, M. M. (2021). Understanding and reducing biases in elite beliefs about the electorate. American Political Science Review, 115(4), 1308–1324. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000305542100037X
  • Pereira, M. M., & Öhberg, P. (2020, October). The expertise curse: How policy expertise can hinder responsiveness. OSF Preprints. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/74kma
  • Pink, S. L., Chu, J., Druckman, J. N., Rand, D. G., & Willer, R. (2021). Elite party cues increase vaccination intentions among republicans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(32), e2106559118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2106559118
  • ProPublica. (2022). Recent congressional statements. 2022. https://projects.propublica.org/represent/statements
  • Rachev, N. R., Han, H., Lacko, D., Gelpı́, R., Yamada, Y., Lieberoth, A., & Aimone, J. A. (2021). Replicating the disease framing problem during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic: A study of stress, worry, trust, and choice under risk. PLOS one, 16(9), e0257151. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257151
  • Rao, A., Morstatter, F., Hu, M., Chen, E., Burghardt, K., Ferrara, E., & Lerman, K. (2021). Political partisanship and antiscience attitudes in online discussions about covid-19: Twitter content analysis. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 23(6), e26692. https://doi.org/10.2196/26692
  • Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The ‘false consensus effect’: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(3), 279–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(77)90049-X
  • Russell, A. (2021). Senate representation on twitter: National policy reputations for constituent communication. Social Science Quarterly, 102(1), 301–323. https://doi.org/10.1111/ssqu.12904
  • Rutjens, B. T., van der Linden, S., & van der Lee, R. (2021). Science skepticism in times of covid-19. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 24(2), 276–283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430220981415
  • Sakakibara, R., & Ozono, H. (2020). Psychological research on the covid-19 crisis in Japan: Focusing on infection preventive behaviors, future prospects, and information dissemination behaviors. Psyarxiv Preprints, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.31234/OSF.IO/97ZYE
  • Salgado, S., & Berntsen, D. (2021). ‘It won’t happen to us’: Unrealistic optimism affects covid-19 risk assessments and attitudes regarding protective behaviour. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 10(3), 368–380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2021.07.006
  • Sheffer, L., John Loewen, P., Soroka, S., Walgrave, S., & Sheafer, T. (2018). Nonrepresentative representatives: An experimental study of the decision making of elected politicians. American Political Science Review, 112(2), 302–321. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055417000569
  • Sheffer, L., & Loewen, P. (2019). Electoral confidence, overconfidence, and risky behavior: Evidence from a study with elected politicians. Political Behavior, 41(1), 31–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9438-0
  • Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of Man, Social and Rational: Mathematical Essays on Rational Human Behavior in Society Setting. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Stecula, D. A., & Pickup, M. (2021). How populism and conservative media fuel conspiracy beliefs about covid-19 and what it means for covid-19 behaviors. Research & Politics, 8(1), 2053168021993979. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168021993979
  • Steffen, J., & Cheng, J. (2021). The influence of gain-loss framing and its interaction with political ideology on social distancing and mask wearing compliance during the covid-19 pandemic. Current Psychology, 42, 8028–8038. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02148-x
  • Sun, L., & Abraham, S. (2021). Estimating dynamic treatment effects in event studies with heterogeneous treatment effects. Journal of Econometrics, 225(2), 175–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeconom.2020.09.006
  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.x
  • Tappin, B. M. (2022). Estimating the between-issue variation in party elite cue effects. Public Opinion Quarterly, 86(4), 862–885. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfac052
  • Tetlock, P. E. (2017). Expert political judgment. Princeton University Press.
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases: Biases in judgments reveal some heuristics of thinking under uncertainty. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
  • Unan, A., Klüver, H., Hobolt, S., & Rodon, T. (2023). The political effects of government communication during crises. OSF Preprints August, 30.
  • Van Vugt, M. (2006). Evolutionary origins of leadership and followership. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(4), 354–371. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_5
  • Visconti, G. (2022). After the flood: Disasters, ideological voting and electoral choices in chile. Political Behavior, 44(4), 1985–2004. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-022-09814-1
  • Wise, T., Zbozinek, T. D., Michelini, G., Hagan, C. C., & Mobbs, D. (2020). Changes in risk perception and self-reported protective behaviour during the first week of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Royal Society Open Science, 7(9), 200742. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200742
  • Wolfe, D., & Dale, D. (2020). All of the times president trump said covid-19 will disappear. Cnn. October 2020. https://edition.cnn.com/interactive/2020/10/politics/covid-disappearing-trump-comment-tracker/
  • Xu, P., & Cheng, J. (2021). Individual differences in social distancing and mask-wearing in the pandemic of covid-19: The role of need for cognition, self-control and risk attitude. Personality and Individual Differences, 175, 110706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110706