401
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Special Section: Sociality, Intersubjectivity, and Social Conflict

Intersubjectivity, Joint Action and Sociality

, &
Pages 904-929 | Received 05 Mar 2019, Accepted 01 Apr 2020, Published online: 01 Sep 2020

References

  • Barbosa, M., Beeghly, M., Moreira, J., Tronick, E., & Fuertes, M. (2018). Robust stability and physiological correlates of infants’ patterns of regulatory behavior in the still-face paradigm at 3 and 9 months. Developmental Psychology, 54(11), 2032–2042. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000616
  • Bardi, L. (2017). Biological motion perception. In B. Hopkins, E. Geangu, & S. Linkenauger (Eds.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of child development (2nd ed., pp. 271–276). Cambridge University Press.
  • Basseches, M., & Mascolo, M. F. (2010). Psychotherapy as a developmental process. Routledge/Taylor & Francis.
  • Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., & Mervis, B. (1986). Mothers’ Internal State Language to Toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 247–261. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1986.tb01016.x
  • Beebe, B., Knoblauch, S., Rustin, J., Sorter, D., Jacobs, T. J., & Pally, R. (2005). Forms of intersubjectivity in infant research and adult treatment. Other Press.
  • Bertenthal, B. (in press). Motor experience and action understanding: A developmental systems perspective. In M. F. Mascolo & T. Bidell (Eds.), Handbook of integrative psychological development. Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
  • Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In M. Lewis and L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. (pp. 29–48). Wiley.
  • Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18(6), 906–921. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.906
  • Bruner, J. (1973). Beyond the information given: Studies in the psychology of knowing. Norton.
  • Burge, T. (2018). Do infants and nonhuman animals attribute mental states? Psychological Review, 125(3), 409–434. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000091
  • Burke, K. (1966). Language as symbolic action. University of California Press.
  • Butt, T. (1998). Sociality, role, and embodiment. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 11(2), 105–116. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/10720539808404643
  • Cipolletta, S. (1998). Costruzione del sé e relazioni familiari. Terapia Familiare, 56(90), 5–22.
  • Cipolletta, S. (2011). Self-construction and interpersonal distances of juveniles living in residential communities. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 24(2), 122–143. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/10720537.2011.548218
  • Cipolletta, S. (2013). Construing in action: Experiencing embodiment. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 26(4), 293–305. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/10720537.2013.812770
  • Cipolletta, S., & Racerro, G. (2003). Testing the serial invalidation hypothesis in the genesis of schizophrenic thought disorder: A research with repertory grids. In G. Chiari & M. L. Nuzzo (Eds.), Psychological constructivism and the social world (pp. 353–368). Franco Angeli.
  • Cipolletta, S., Sommacale, S., Nucci, M., & Faccio, E. (2020). What is the role of similarity and understanding in lesbian couples’ satisfaction? A constructivist analysis. Under Review.
  • Craig, G., & Duck, S. W. (1977). Similarity, interpersonal attitudes and attraction: The evaluative-descriptive distinction. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 15–21. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1977.tb00998.x
  • DeCasper, A. J., & Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects on learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behavior & Development, 4, 19–35.
  • De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2008). Making sense in participation: An enactive approach to social cognition. In F. Morganti, A. Carassa, G. Riva, F. Morganti, A. Carassa, G. Riva (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective on the study of interactions (pp. 33–47). IOS Press.
  • di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176–180. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230027
  • Duck, S. W. (1975). Personality similarity and friendship choices by adolescents. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5(3), 351–383. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420050308
  • Duck, S. W., & Spencer, C. (1972). Personal constructs and friendship formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23(1), 40–45. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032872
  • Falck-Ytter, T., Bakker, M., & von Hofsten, C. (2011). Human infants orient to biological motion rather than audiovisual synchrony. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 2131–2135. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.040
  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87(6), 477–531. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.87.6.477
  • Fivaz-Depeursinge, E., & Corboz-Warnery, J. (1999). The primary triangle. Basic Books.
  • Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships. University of Chicago Press.
  • Goldman, A. I. (2012). Theory of mind. In E. Margolis, R. Samuels, & S. P. Stich (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of cognitive Science (pp. 402–424). Oxford University Press.
  • Goubet, N., & Clifton, R. K. (1998). Object and event representation in 6½-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 63–76. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.63
  • Green, D., Li, Q., Lockman, J. J., & Gredebäck, G. (2016). Culture influences action understanding in infancy: Prediction of actions performed with chopsticks and spoons in Chinese and Swedish infants. Child Development, 87(3), 736–746. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12500
  • Hacker, P. M. S. (2012). The relevance of Wittgenstein’s philosophy of psychology to the psychological sciences. In P. Stekeler-Weithofer (Ed.), Proceedings of the Leipzig conference on Wittgenstein and science 2007 (pp. 205–223). Meiner.
  • Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: The role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 614.
  • Hamlin, J. K. (2015). The case for social evaluation in preverbal infants: Gazing toward one's goal drives infants' preferences for Helpers over Hinderers in the hill paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1563. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01563
  • Hart, S. L. (2015). Jealousy in infants: Laboratory research on differential treatment. Springer International Publishing.
  • Harter, S., Neimeyer, R. A., & Alexander, P. C. (1989). Personal construction of family relationships: The relation of commonality and sociality to family satisfaction for parents and adolescent. International Journal of Personal Construct Psychology, 2(2), 123–142. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/08936038908406104
  • Iacoboni, M. (2009). The problem of other minds is not a problem: Mirror neurons and intersubjectivity. In J. A. Pineda (Ed.), Mirror neuron systems: The role of mirroring processes in social cognition (pp. 121–133). Humana Press.
  • Kärtner, J., Keller, H., Chaudhary, N., & Yovsi, R. D. (2012). The development of mirror self-recognition in different sociocultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77(4), 1–101.
  • Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs, vols. 1 and 2. Norton.
  • Kelmanson, I. A. (2012). Separation anxiety and bedtime resistance in eight-month-old infants. Early Child Development and Care, 182(11), 1455–1464. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2011.619659
  • Kilner, J. M., & Lemon, R. N. (2013). What we know currently about mirror neurons. Current Biology : CB, 23(23), R1057–R1062. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.051
  • Lau, I. Y.-M., Lee, S.-I., & Chiu, C. (2004). Language, cognition, and reality: Constructing shared meanings through communication. In M. Schaller & C. S. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 77–100). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Luo, Y., & Choi, Y. (2013). Infants attribute mental states to nonhuman agents. In M. D. Rutherford & V. A. Kuhlmeier (Eds.), Social perception: Detection and interpretation of animacy, agency, and intention (pp. 259–281). MIT Press.
  • Macdonald, C. (2014). In my “mind’s eye”: Introspectionism, detectivism, and the basis of authoritative self-knowledge. Synthese, 191(15), 3685–3710. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0487-1
  • Mascolo, M. F. (2009). Wittgenstein and the discursive analysis of emotion. New Ideas in Psychology, 27(2), 258–274. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2008.04.004
  • Mascolo, M. F. (2013). Developing through relationships: An embodied coactive systems framework. In Lerner, R., & Benson, J. (Eds.), Embodiment and epigenesis: Theoretical and methodological issues in understanding the role of biology within the relational developmental system. Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 45, pp. 185–222).Elsevier.
  • Mascolo, M. F. (2017). How objectivity undermines the study of personhood: Toward an intersubjective epistemology for psychological science. New Ideas in Psychology, 44, 41–48. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2016.11.005
  • Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Developmental transformations in appraisals for pride, guilt and shame. In K. W. Fischer & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (pp. 64–113). Guilford.
  • Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (1998). The development of self through the coordination of component systems. In M. Ferrari & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Self-awareness: Its nature and development (pp. 332–384). Guilford.
  • Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (2010). The dynamic development of thinking, feeling and acting over the lifespan. In R. Lerner & W. Overton (Eds.), Handbook of lifespan development (Vol. 1, (pp. 149–194). John Wiley.
  • Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (2015). Dynamic development of thinking, feeling and acting. In W. Overton & P. Molennar (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (Vol 1: Theory and method) (pp. 113–161). John Wiley.
  • Mascolo, M. F., & Kallio, E. (2019). Beyond free will: The embodied emergence of conscious agency. Philosophical Psychology, 32, 437–462.
  • Mascolo, M. F., & Kallio, E. (2020). The phenomenology of between: An intersubjective epistemology for psychological science. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 33(1), 1–28.
  • Matusov, E. (1996). Intersubjectivity without agreement. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 3, 25–45.
  • McCune, L. (2010). Developing symbolic abilities. In B. Wagoner (Ed.), Symbolic transformation: The mind in movement through culture and society (pp. 193–208). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2013). Origins of social cognition: Bidirectional self-other mapping and the 'like-me' hypothesis. In M. R. Banaji, S. A. Gelman, M. R. Banaji, S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 139–144). Oxford University Press.
  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54(3), 702–709. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2307/1130058
  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, K. M. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702.
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). “Like me”: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10(1), 126–134.
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2013). Origins of social cognition: Bidirectional self-other mapping and the “like-me” hypothesis. In M. R. Banaji & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 139–144). Oxford University Press.
  • Mendoza, O. (1985). The exchange grid. In N. Beail (Ed.), Repertory grid technique: Application in clinical and educational settings (pp. 173–189). Croom Helm.
  • Nagy, E., Pilling, K., Watt, R., Pal, A., & Orvos, H. (2017). Neonates’ responses to repeated exposure to a still face. PLoS One, 12(8), e0181688. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181688
  • Namy, L. L. (Ed.). (2005). Symbol use and symbolic representation: Developmental and comparative perspectives. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Neimeyer, G. J., & Hudson, J. E. (1985). Couples’ constructs: Personal systems in marital satisfaction. In D. Bannister (Ed.), Issues and approaches in personal construct theory (pp. 127–142). Academic Press.
  • Neimeyer, G. J., & Neimeyer, R. A. (1981). Functional similarity and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Research in Personality, 15(4), 427–435. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(81)90039-8
  • Nelson, K., & Shaw, L. K. (2002). Developing a socially shared symbolic system. In E. Amsel & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Language, literacy, and cognitive development: The development and consequences of symbolic communication (pp. 27–57). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Procter, H. G. (1985). Repertory grid techniques in family therapy and research. In N. Beail (Ed.), Repertory grid technique: Application in clinical and educational settings (pp. 218–242). Croom Helm.
  • Procter, H. G. (2002). Constructs of individuals and relationships. Context, 59, 11–12.
  • Procter, H. G. (2014). Qualitative grids, the relationality corollary and the levels of interpersonal construing. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 27 (4), 243–226. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/10720537.2013.820655
  • Procter, H. G., & Winter, D. A. (2020). Personal and relational construct psychotherapy. Palgrave-Macmillan.
  • Racine, T. P., & Carpendale, J. M. (2008). The embodiment of mental states. In W. F. Overton, U. Müller, J. L. Newman, W. F. Overton, U. Müller, & J. L. Newman (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on embodiment and consciousness (pp. 159–190). Taylor & Francis Group/Lawrence.
  • Razuri, E. B., Howard, A. R. H., Purvis, K. B., & Cross, D. R. (2017). Mental state language development: The longitudinal roles of attachment and maternal language. Infant Mental Health Journal, 38(3), 329–342. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21638
  • Rochat, P., & Passos-Ferreira, C. (2009). From imitation to reciprocation and mutual recognition. In J. A. Pineda (Ed.), Mirror neuron systems: The role of mirroring processes in social cognition (pp. 191–212). Humana Press.
  • Rochat, P., Querido, J. G., & Striano, T. (1999). Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of proto-conversation in early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 950–957. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.950
  • Salla, M., Feixas, G., Ballén, C., Muñoz, D., & Compañ, V. (2015). The couple's grid: A tool for assessing interpersonal construction in couples. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 28(1), 53–66. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/10720537.2013.859110
  • Sherover, C. M. (1987). Royce’s pragmatic idealism and existential phenomenology. In R. S. Corrington, C. Hausman, & T. M. Seebohm, (Eds.), Pragmatism considers phenomenology (pp. 143–165).The Center for Advanced Research in Phenomenology.
  • Short, T. (2015). Simulation theory: A psychological and philosophical consideration. Psychology Press.
  • Shotter, J. (2000). Wittgenstein and the everyday: From radical hiddenness to “nothing is hidden”; From representation to participation. Journal of Mundane Behavior, 1, 116–134.
  • Shotter, J. (2011, March 5–9). Language, joint action, and the ethical domain: The importance of the relations between our living bodies and their surroundings. In Plenary paper given at IIIrd Congreso de Psicologia y Responsabilidad Social, Campus San Alberto Magno, Columbia.
  • Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2016). Self-awareness moderates the relation between maternal mental state language about desires and children’s mental state vocabulary. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 114–129. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.012
  • Ter Hark, M. (1990). Beyond the inner and the outer: Wittgenstein’s philosophy of psychology. Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  • Thomas, L. F. (1979). Constructs, reflect and converse: The conversational reconstruction of social realities. In P. Stringer & D. Bannister (Eds.), Constructs of sociality and individuality (pp. 49–72). Academic Press.
  • Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.
  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). Cambridge University Press.
  • Trevarthen, C. (1990). Signs before speech. In T. A. Sebeok & J. Umiker–Sebeok (Eds.), The semiotic web (pp. 689–755). Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Trevarthen, C. (1993). The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 121–173). Cambridge University Press.
  • Trevarthen, C. (2001). Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: Their origin, development and significance for infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22(1–2), 95–131. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355(200101/04)22:1<95::AID-IMHJ4>3.0.CO;2-6
  • Trevarthen, C. (2009). The functions of emotion in infancy: The regulation and communication of rhythm, sympathy, and meaning in human development. In D. Fosha, D. J. Siegel, M. F. Solomon, D. Fosha, D. J. Siegel, & M. F. Solomon (Eds.), The healing power of emotion: Affective neuroscience, development & clinical practice (pp. 55–85). Norton.
  • Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(1), 3–48. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00701
  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary Intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol (pp. 183–229). Academic Press.
  • Ugazio, V. (2013). Semantic polarities and psychopathologies in the family: Permitted and forbidden stories. Routledge.
  • Ugazio, V., & Guanieri, S. (2017). The family semantics grid II: Narrated polarities in couples. Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 24(2), 1–39.
  • Verhagan, A. (2008). Intersubjectivity and the architecture of the language system. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha, E. Itkonen, J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 307–331). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J., & Jackson, D. D. (1968). Pragmatics of human communication. Norton.
  • Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as action. Oxford University Press.
  • Wellman, H. M. (2012). Theory of mind: Better methods, clearer findings, more development. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 313–330.
  • Wertsch, J. V. (2007). Mediation. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky (pp. 178–192). Cambridge University Press.
  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Blackwell.
  • Zahavi, D. (2006). Subjectivity and selfhood. MIT Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.