270
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Note

Isolating Proactive Slowing from Reactive Inhibitory Control in Heavy Drinkers

ORCID Icon, , &

References

  • Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), e55–e68. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024
  • Bednarski, S. R., Erdman, E., Luo, X., Zhang, S., Hu, S., & Li, C. S. (2012). Neural processes of an indirect analog of risk taking in young nondependent adult alcohol drinkers-an FMRI study of the stop signal task. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 36(5), 768–779. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01672.x
  • Berger, I., Slobodin, O., & Cassuto, H. (2017). Usefulness and validity of continuous performance tests in the diagnosis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32(1), 81–93. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw101
  • Bø, R., & Landro, N. I. (2017). Inhibitory control and response monitoring are not systematically related to weekly alcohol consumption in the general population. Psychopharmacology, 234(11), 1761–1768. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4578-9
  • Brevers, D., Bechara, A., Kilts, C., Antoniali, V., Bruylant, A., Verbanck, P., … Noël, X. (2017). Competing motivations: Proactive response inhibition toward addiction-related stimuli in quitting-motivated individuals. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34(3), 785–806.
  • Chatham, C. H., Frank, M. J., & Munakata, Y. (2009). Pupillometric and behavioral markers of a developmental shift in the temporal dynamics of cognitive control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(14), 5529–5533. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810002106
  • Christiansen, P., Cole, J. C., Goudie, A. J., & Field, M. (2012). Components of behavioural impulsivity and automatic cue approach predict unique variance in hazardous drinking. Psychopharmacology, 219(2), 501–510. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2396-z
  • Colder, C. R., & O'Connor, R. (2002). Attention bias and disinhibited behavior as predictors of alcohol use and enhancement reasons for drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(4), 325–332. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.16.4.325
  • Collins, R. L., & Lapp, W. M. (1992). The temptation and restraint Inventory for measuring drinking restraint. Addiction, 87(4), 625–633. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1992.tb01964.x
  • Czapla, M., Simon, J. J., Friederich, H. C., Herpertz, S. C., Zimmermann, P., & Loeber, S. (2015). Is binge drinking in young adults associated with an alcohol-specific impairment of response inhibition? European Addiction Research, 21(2), 105–113. doi: 10.1159/000367939
  • de Wit, H. (2009). Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: A review of underlying processes. Addiction Biology, 14(1), 22–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x
  • Fernie, G., Cole, J. C., Goudie, A. J., & Field, M. (2010). Risk-taking but not response inhibition or delay discounting predict alcohol consumption in social drinkers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 112(1-2), 54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.05.011
  • Fillmore, M. T. (2003). Drug abuse as a problem of impaired control: Current approaches and findings. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2(3), 179–197. doi: 10.1177/1534582303257007
  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101
  • Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2002). Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: Neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(10), 1642–1652. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642
  • Gonthier, C., Macnamara, B. N., Chow, M., Conway, A. R. A., & Braver, T. S. (2016). Inducing proactive control shifts in the AX-CPT. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1822–1822. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01822
  • Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(9), 651–659. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897
  • Houston, R. J., Derrick, J. L., Leonard, K. E., Testa, M., Quigley, B. M., & Kubiak, A. (2014). Effects of heavy drinking on executive cognitive functioning in a community sample. Addictive Behaviors, 39(1), 345–349. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.032
  • Jones, A., & Field, M. (2015). Alcohol-related and negatively-valenced cues increase motor and oculomotor disinhibition in social drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 23(2), 122–129. doi: 10.1037/pha0000011
  • Lesh, T. A., Westphal, A. J., Niendam, T. A., Yoon, J. H., Minzenberg, M. J., Ragland, J. D., … Carter, C. S. (2013). Proactive and reactive cognitive control and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia. Neuroimage: Clinical, 2, 590–599. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.04.010
  • Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta-experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 102–111. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.1.102
  • McEvoy, P. M., Stritzke, W. G. K., French, D. J., Lang, A. R., & Ketterman, R. L. (2004). Comparison of three models of alcohol craving in young adults: A cross-validation. Addiction, 99(4), 482–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00714.x
  • Murphy, P., & Garavan, H. (2011). Cognitive predictors of problem drinking and AUDIT scores among college students. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 115(1–2), 94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.10.011
  • Nederkoorn, C., Baltus, M., Guerrieri, R., & Wiers, R. W. (2009). Heavy drinking is associated with deficient response inhibition in women but not in men. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93(3), 331–336. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.015
  • Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1
  • Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Racine, C. A., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1010–1028. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm135
  • Paz, A. L., Keim, C. A., & Rosselli, M. (2016). Inhibitory performance predicting drinking behaviours among young adults. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 51(6), 677–683. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agw017
  • Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2011). Rapid communication: Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(6), 1048–1055. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.577226
  • Riggs, N. R., & Pentz, M. A. (2016). Inhibitory control and the onset of combustible cigarette, e-cigarette, and hookah use in early adolescence: The moderating role of socioeconomic status. Child Neuropsychology, 22(6), 679–691. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1053389
  • Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D., Jr., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20(5), 343–350. doi: 10.1037/h0043220
  • Rubio, G., Jiménez, M., Rodríguez-Jiménez, R., Martínez, I., Ávila, C., Ferre, F., … Palomo, T. (2008). The role of behavioral impulsivity in the development of alcohol dependence: A 4-year follow-up study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32(9), 1681–1687. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00746.x
  • Rush, B. K., Barch, D. M., & Braver, T. S. (2006). Accounting for cognitive aging: Context processing, inhibition or processing speed? Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13(3–4), 588–610. doi: 10.1080/13825580600680703
  • Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., De la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption II. Addiction, 88(6), 791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x
  • Smith, J. L., Mattick, R. P., Jamadar, S. D., & Iredale, J. M. (2014). Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: A meta-analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 145, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.08.009
  • Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1990). Self-report issues in alcohol abuse: State of the art and future directions. Behavioral Assessment, 12(1), 77–90.
  • Verbruggen, F., Aron, A. R., Band, G. P., Beste, C., Bissett, P. G., Brockett, A. T., … Boehler, C. N. (2019). A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. eLife, 8, e46323. doi: 10.7554/eLife.46323
  • Verbruggen, F., & De Houwer, J. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 21(2), 391–403. doi: 10.1080/02699930600625081
  • Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009a). Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(5), 647–661. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.014
  • Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009b). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835–854. doi: 10.1037/a0012726
  • Verbruggen, F., McLaren, I. P. L., & Chambers, C. D. (2014). Banishing the control homunculi in studies of action control and behavior change. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(5), 497–524. doi: 10.1177/1745691614526414
  • Verbruggen, F., Stevens, T., & Chambers, C. D. (2014). Proactive and reactive stopping when distracted: An attentional account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1295–1300. doi: 10.1037/a0036542
  • Yucel, M., Oldenhof, E., Ahmed, S., Belin, D., Billieux, J., Bowden-Jones, H. … Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2019). A transdiagnostic dimensional approach towards a neuropsychological assessment for addiction: An international Delphi consensus study. Addiction, 114(6), 1095–1109. doi: 10.1111/add.14424
  • Zandbelt, B. B., van Buuren, M., Kahn, R. S., & Vink, M. (2011). Reduced proactive inhibition in schizophrenia is related to corticostriatal dysfunction and poor working memory. Biological Psychiatry, 70(12), 1151–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.028

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.