545
Views
20
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

On Sources of the Word Length Effect in Young Readers

, &

REFERENCES

  • Aghababian, V., & Nazir, T. A. (2000). Developing normal reading skills: Aspects of the visual processes underlying word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 123–150. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2540
  • Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283–316. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2007). The lme4 package (R Package, Version 2). Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package_lme4
  • Bijeljac-Babic, R., Millogo, V., Farioli, F., & Grainger, J. (2004). A developmental investigation of word length effects in reading using a new on-line word identification paradigm. Reading and Writing, 17, 411–431. doi:10.1023/B:READ.0000032664.20755.af
  • Blythe, H. I., Häikiö, T., Bertam, R., Liversedge, S. P., & Hyönä, J. (2011). Reading disappearing text: Why do children refixate words? Vision Research, 51, 84–92. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.003
  • Bosch, E., Rabenstein, R., & Schorch, G. (1983). Erhebungen zum Erstschreibunterricht nach dem bayerischen Grundschullehrplan ‘81. Nürnberg, Germany: Inst. für Grundschulforschung d. Univ. Erlangen-Nürnberg.
  • Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 156–178. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(90)90037-9
  • Brysbaert, M., Buchmeier, M., Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. M., Bölte, J., & Böhl, A. (2011). The word frequency effect: A review of recent developments and implications for the choice of frequency estimates in German. Experimental Psychology, 58, 412–424. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000123
  • Davies, R., Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., Suárez, P., & Cuetos, F. (2013). Lexical and sub-lexical effects on accuracy, reaction time and response duration: Impaired and typical word and pseudoword reading in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 26, 721–738. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9388-1
  • De Luca, M., Burani, C., Paizi, D., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2010). Letter and letter-string processing in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 46, 1272–1283. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.06.007
  • Gagl, B., Hawelka, S., & Hutzler, F. (2014). A similar correction mechanism in slow and fluent readers after suboptimal landing positions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 355. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00355
  • Hawelka, S., Gagl, B., & Wimmer, H. (2010). A dual-route perspective on eye movements ofdyslexic readers. Cognition, 115, 367–379. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.004
  • Huestegge, L., Radach, R., Corbic, D., & Huestegge, S. M. (2009). Oculomotor and linguistic determinants of reading development: A longitudinal study. Vision Research, 49, 2948–2959. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.012
  • Kliegl, R., Wei, P., Dambacher, M., Yan, M., & Zhou, X. (2010). Experimental effects and individual differences in linear mixed models: Estimating the relationship between spatial, object, and attraction effects in visual attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 1.
  • Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315–334. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00005-X
  • MacKeben, M., Trauzettel-Klosinski, S., Reinhard, J., Durrwachter, U., Adler, M., & Klosinski, G. (2004). Eye movement control during single-word reading in dyslexics. Journal of Vision, 4(5), 388–402. doi:10.1167/4.5.4
  • Marinus, E., & de Jong, P. F. (2010). Variability in the word-reading performance of dyslexic readers: Effects of letter length, phoneme length and digraph presence. Cortex, 46, 1259–1271. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.005
  • Marinus, E., de Jong, P., & van der Leij, A. (2012). Increasing word-reading speed in poor readers: No additional benefits of explicit letter-cluster training. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 166–185. doi:10.1080/10888438.2011.554471
  • Martelli, M., De Luca, M., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C., Pontillo, M., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2014). Bridging the gap between different measures of the reading speed deficit in developmental dyslexia. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 237–252. doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3735-6
  • Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic and normal reading children. Brain and Language, 98, 140–149. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.003
  • Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Salzburger lese-screening für die klassenstufen 1-4 (SLS 1-4). Bern, Switzerland: Verlag Hans Huber.
  • Rau, A., Möller, K., & Landerl, K. (2014). The transition from sublexical to lexical processing in a consistent orthography: An eye-tracking study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 224–233. doi:10.1080/10888438.2013.857673
  • Rayner, K. (1975). The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 65–81. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(75)90005-5
  • Thaler, V., Ebner, E. M., Wimmer, H., & Landerl, K. (2004). Training reading fluency in dysfluent readers with high reading accuracy: Word specific effects but low transfer to untrained words. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 89–113. doi:10.1007/s11881-004-0005-0
  • Thaler, V., Urton, K., Heine, A., Hawelka, S., Engl, V., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Different behavioral and eye movement patterns of dyslexic readers with and without attentional deficits during single word reading. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2436–2445. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.006
  • Treiman, R. (1986). The division between onsets and rimes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 476–491. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(86)90039-2
  • Treiman, R. (1991). Children’s spelling errors on syllable-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 346–360. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.3.346
  • Treiman, R., & Weatherston, S. (1992). Effects of linguistic structure on children’s ability to isolate initial consonants. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 174–181. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.2.174
  • Van Den Boer, M., de Jong, P. F., & Haentjens-van Meeteren, M. M. (2012). Lexical decision in children: Sublexical processing or lexical search? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1214–1228. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.652136
  • Yarkoni, T., Balota, D., & Yap, M. (2008). Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 971–979.
  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193. doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5
  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Gasperini, F., Judica, A., & Spinelli, D. (2005). Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 93, 369–373. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.010

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.