878
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Automatic Activation of Sounds by Letters Occurs Early in Development but is not Impaired in Children With Dyslexia

References

  • Aravena, S., Snellings, P., Tijms, J., & Van Der Molen, M. W. (2013). A lab-controlled simulation of a letter–speech sound binding deficit in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 691–707. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.009
  • Bishop, D. (2007). Using mismatch negativity to study central auditory processing in developmental language and literacy impairments: Where are we, and where should we be going? Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 651. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.651
  • Blau, V., Reithler, J., Van Atteveldt, N., Seitz, J., Gerretsen, P., Goebel, R., & Blomert, L. (2010). Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain, 133(3), 868–879. doi:10.1093/brain/awp308
  • Blau, V., Van Atteveldt, N., Ekkebus, M., Goebel, R., & Blomert, L. (2009). Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds links phonological and reading deficits in adult dyslexia. Current Biology, 19(6), 503–508. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.065
  • Blomert, L. (2011). The neural signature of orthographic–phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. Neuroimage, 57(3), 695–703. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.003
  • Blomert, L., & Froyen, D. (2010). Multi-sensory learning and learning to read. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 77(3), 195–204. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.06.025
  • Blomert, L., & Willems, G. (2010). Is there a causal link from a phonological awareness deficit to reading failure in children at familial risk for dyslexia? Dyslexia, 16(4), 300–317. doi:10.1002/dys.v16:4
  • Boets, B., Op de Beeck, H. P., Vandermosten, M., Scott, S. K., Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D., … Ghesquière, P. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science, 342(6163), 1251–1254.
  • Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Defior, S., Málková, G. S., & Hulme, C. (2013). Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1398–1407. doi:10.1177/0956797612473122
  • Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavský, M., Onochie-Quintanilla, E., … Hulme, C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678–686. doi:10.1177/0956797611434536
  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91(1), 77–111.
  • Cohen Kadosh, R., Cohen Kadosh, K., Henik, A., & Linden, D. E. J. (2008). Processing conflicting information: Facilitation, interference, and functional connectivity. Neuropsychologia, 46(12), 2872–2879. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.025
  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1980). The influence of orthography on readers’ conceptualization of the phonemic structure of words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1(4), 371–385.
  • Elbro, C., Borstrøm, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 36–60. doi:10.1598/RRQ.33.1.3
  • Ellis, D. (2010). Time-domain scrambling of audio signals in Matlab. Retrieved from http://www.ee.columbia.edu/~dpwe/resources/matlab/scramble/
  • Foster, H. (2007). Single Word Reading Test 6-16. London, UK: GL Assessment Limited.
  • Froyen, D. J., Bonte, M. L., Van Atteveldt, N., & Blomert, L. (2009). The long road to automation: Neurocognitive development of letter–speech sound processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 567–580. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21061
  • Froyen, D. J., Willems, G., & Blomert, L. (2011). Evidence for a specific cross‐modal association deficit in dyslexia: An electrophysiological study of letter–speech sound processing. Developmental Science, 14(4), 635–648. doi:10.1111/desc.2011.14.issue-4
  • Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: A comparison between Scandinavian and US/Australian children. Dyslexia, 16(2), 119–142.
  • Griffiths, Y. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2002). Predictors of exception word and nonword reading in dyslexic children: The severity hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 34–43. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.34
  • Hulme, C., Bowyer-Crane, C., Carroll, J. M., Duff, F. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2012). The causal role of phoneme awareness and letter-sound knowledge in learning to read combining intervention studies with mediation analyses. Psychological Science, 23(6), 572–577. doi:10.1177/0956797611435921
  • Hulme, C., Caravolas, M., Málková, G., & Brigstocke, S. (2005). Phoneme isolation ability is not simply a consequence of letter-sound knowledge. Cognition, 97(1), B1–11. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.002
  • Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2013). Learning to read: What we know and what we need to understand better. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 1–5. doi:10.1111/cdep.12005
  • Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2014). The interface between spoken and written language: Developmental disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120395. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0395
  • Hulme, C., Stothard, S., Clarke, P., Bowyer-Crane, C., Harrington, A., Truelove, E., & Snowling, M. (2009). YARC york assessment of reading for comprehension. London, UK: University of York, Centre for Reading and Language: GL Assessment.
  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt.
  • Kronschnabel, J., Brem, S., Maurer, U., & Brandeis, D. (2014). The level of audiovisual print–speech integration deficits in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 62, 245–261. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.024
  • Landerl, K. (2001). Word recognition deficits in German: More evidence from a representative sample. Dyslexia, 7(4), 183–196. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0909
  • Langdon, R., & Smith, P. (2005). Spatial cueing by social versus nonsocial directional signals. Visual Cognition, 12(8), 1497–1527. doi:10.1080/13506280444000805
  • Lervåg, A., Bråten, I., & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 764–781. doi:10.1037/a0014132
  • Litt, R. A., & Nation, K. (2014). The nature and specificity of paired associate learning deficits in children with dyslexia. Journal of Memory and Language, 71(1), 71–88. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.10.005
  • Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322–352. doi:10.1037/a0026744
  • Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 297–326. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297
  • Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Reason, 4(2), 61–64.
  • Nash, H. M., Gooch, D., Hulme, C., Mahajan, Y., McArthur, G., Steinmetzger, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2016). Are the literacy difficulties that characterize developmental dyslexia associated with a failure to integrate letters and speech sounds? Developmental Science. doi:10.1111/desc.12423
  • Nicolson, R. I. (1994). Reaction times and dyslexia. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47(1), 29–48. doi:10.1080/14640749408401142
  • Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2015). Developmental Dyslexia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11(1), 283–307. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842
  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. (2004). Attention and cognitive control. In D. A. Balota & E. J. Marsh (Eds.), Cognitive psychology: Key readings (pp. 205–223). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., MacCallum, R. C., & Nicewander, W. A. (2005). Use of the extreme groups approach: A critical reexamination and new recommendations. Psychological Methods, 10(2), 178–192. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.10.2.178
  • Roelofs, A., Van Turennout, M., & Coles, M. G. H. (2006). Anterior cingulate cortex activity can be independent of response conflict in Stroop-like tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(37), 13884–13889. doi:10.1073/pnas.0606265103
  • Selst, M. V., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(3), 631–650. doi:10.1080/14640749408401131
  • Taylor, J., Plunkett, K., & Nation, K. (2011). The influence of consistency, frequency, and semantics on learning to read: An artificial orthography paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 60.
  • Thompson, P. A., Hulme, C., Nash, H. M., Gooch, D., Hayiou‐Thomas, E., & Snowling, M. J. (2015). Developmental dyslexia: Predicting individual risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12412
  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. (1999). TOWRE–2 Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
  • Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2006). Predicting delayed letter knowledge development and its relation to grade 1 reading achievement among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1128. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1128
  • Van Atteveldt, N., & Ansari, D. (2014). How symbols transform brain function: A review in memory of Leo Blomert. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 3(2), 44–49. doi:10.1016/j.tine.2014.04.001
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-II UK): Harcourt assessment. London, UK: Pearson Assessment.
  • Widmann, A., Schröger, E., Tervaniemi, M., Pakarinen, S., & Kujala, T. (2012). Mapping symbols to sounds: Electrophysiological correlates of the impaired reading process in dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 60.
  • Wimmer, H., & Schurz, M. (2010). Dyslexia in regular orthographies: Manifestation and causation. Dyslexia, 16(4), 283–299.
  • Žarić, G., González, G. F., Tijms, J., Van Der Molen, M. W., Blomert, L., & Bonte, M. (2014). Reduced neural integration of letters and speech sounds in Dyslexic children scales with individual differences in reading fluency. PLoS One, 9(10), 1–14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0110337

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.