4,504
Views
13
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Morphological Processing Before and During Children’s Spelling

References

  • Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and pen: A new device for studying reading during writing. Behavior Research Methods, 38(2), 287–299.
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R Package Version, 1.1-7.
  • Bonin, P., Méot, A., Lagarrigue, A., & Roux, S. (2015). Written object naming, spelling to dictation, and immediate copying: Different tasks, different pathways? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(7), 1268–1294. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.978877
  • Breadmore, H. L., & Carroll, J. M. (2016). Morphological spelling in spite of phonological deficits: Evidence from children with dyslexia and otitis media. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(6), 1439–1460. doi:10.1017/S0142716416000072
  • Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247–266.
  • Deacon, S. H. (2008). The metric matters: Determining the extent of children’s knowledge of morphological spelling regularities. Developmental Science, 11(3), 396–406. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00684.x
  • Deacon, S. H., & Bryant, P. (2006a). Getting to the root: Young writers’ sensitivity to the role of root morphemes in the spelling of inflected and derived words. Journal of Child Language, 33(02), 401. doi:10.1017/s0305000906007409
  • Deacon, S. H., & Bryant, P. (2006b). This turnip’s not for turning: Children’s morphological awareness and their use of root morphemes in spelling. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 567–575. doi:10.1348/026151005x50834
  • Deacon, S. H., Conrad, N., & Pacton, S. (2008). A statistical learning perspective on children’s learning about graphotactic and morphological regularities in spelling. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49(2), 118–124. doi:10.1037/0708-5591.49.2.118
  • Deacon, S. H., & Dhooge, S. (2010). Developmental stability and changes in the impact of root consistency on children’s spelling. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 23(9), 1055–1069. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9195-5
  • Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18(2), 116–125. doi:10.1111/jrir.1995.18.issue-2
  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theories, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4
  • Gentry, J. R. (1982). An analysis of developmental spelling in GNYS AT WRK. The Reading Teacher, 36, 192–200.
  • Grainger, J., Lété, B., Bertand, D., Dufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition, 123(2), 280–292. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.003
  • Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(54), 1–13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054
  • Hanna, P. R., Hanna, J. S., Hodges, R. E., & Rudorf, E. H. (1966). Phoneme-grapheme correspondences as cues to spelling improvement. Office of Education; US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662–720. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.662
  • Kandel, S., Álvarez, C. J., & Vallée, N. (2008). Morphemes also serve as processing units in handwriting production. In M. Baciu (Ed.), Neuropsychology and cognition of language behavioural, neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies of spoken and written language (pp. 87–100). Kerala, India: Research Signpost.
  • Kandel, S., Peereman, R., & Ghimenton, A. (2013). Further evidence for the interaction of central and peripheral processes: The impact of double letters in writing English words. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 729. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00729
  • Kandel, S., Spinelli, E., Tremblay, A., Guerassimovitch, H., & Álvarez, C. J. (2012). Processing prefixes and suffixes in handwriting production. Acta Psychologica, 140(3), 187–195. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.04.005
  • Lambert, E., Alamargot, D., Larocque, D., & Caporossi, G. (2011). Dynamics of the spelling process during a copy task: Effects of regularity and frequency. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 65(3), 141–150. doi:10.1037/a0022538
  • Morris, D., & Perney, J. (1984). Developmental spelling as a predictor of first-grade reading achievement. The Elementary School Journal, 84(4), 441–457. doi:10.1086/461375
  • Nation, K. (2009). Form–Meaning links in the development of visual word recognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3665–3674. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0119
  • Olive, T. (2014). Toward a parallel and cascading model of the writing system: A review of research on writing processes coordination. Journal of Writing Research, 6(2), 173–194. doi:10.17239/jowr-2014.06.02.4
  • Ouellette, G., & Sénéchal, M. (2008). Pathways to literacy: A study of invented spelling and its role in learning to read. Child Development, 79(4), 899–913. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01166.x
  • Pacton, S., & Deacon, S. H. (2008). The timing and mechanisms of children’s use of morphological information in spelling: A review of evidence from English and French. Cognitive Development, 23(3), 339–359. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.09.004
  • Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357–383. doi:10.1080/10888430701530730
  • Quémart, P., & Lambert, E. (2017). The influence of task constraints on morphological processing during written word production. Paper presented at the 10th International Morphological Procesing Conference (MoProc), SISSA, Trieste, Italy.
  • Quémart, P., & Lambert, E. (2018). The influence of the morphological structure of words on the dynamics of handwriting in adults and fourth and sixth grade children. Reading and Writing an Interdisciplinary Journal. doi:10.1007/s11145-017-9762-0
  • Rashotte, C. A., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Publishing, Inc.
  • Sawi, O. M., & Rueckl, J. (2018). Reading and the neurocognitive bases of statistical learning. Scientific Studies of Reading, Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/10888438.2018.1457681
  • Team, R. 2016. RStudio: Integrated development for R. (Version Version 1.0.136). Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc. Retrieved from http://www.rstudio.com/
  • Treiman, R. (1998). Why Spelling? The benefits of incorporating spelling into beginning reading instruction. In J. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 289). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Treiman, R. (2017). Learning to spell: Phonology and beyond. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(3–4), 83–93. doi:10.1080/02643294.2017.1337630
  • Treiman, R., & Cassar, M. (1996). Effects of morphology on children’s spelling of final consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 141–170.
  • Treiman, R., Cassar, M., & Zukowski, A. (1994). What types of linguistic information do children use in spelling? The case of flaps. Child Development, 65, 1310–1329. doi:10.2307/1131501
  • Van Galen, G. P. (1991). Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory. Human Movement Science, 10(2–3), 165–191. doi:10.1016/0167-9457(91)90003-G
  • Van Heuven, W. J. B., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1176–1190. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.850521