137
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The insistence of vision: Why do people look at a salient stimulus when it signals target absence?

, &
Pages 1122-1157 | Received 30 Sep 2009, Accepted 08 Aug 2011, Published online: 18 Nov 2011

References

  • Anderson , J. R. and Lebiere , C. 1998 . The atomic components of thought , Mahwah, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates .
  • Ballard , D. H. , Hayhoe , M. M. , Pook , P. K. and Rao , R. P. N. 1997 . Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition . Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 20 ( 4 ) : 723 – 742 .
  • Boot , W. R. , McCarley , J. S. , Kramer , A. F. and Peterson , M. S. 2004 . Automatic and intentional memory processes in visual search . Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , 11 ( 2 ) : 854 – 861 .
  • Braver , T. S. , Gray , J. R. and Burgess , G. C. 2007 . “ Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control ” . In Variation in working memory , Edited by: Conway , A. R. A. , Jarrold , C. , Kane , M. J. , Miyake , A. and Towse , J. N. 76 – 106 . New York, NY : Oxford University Press .
  • Butler , K. M. , Zacks , R. T. and Henderson , J. M. 1999 . Suppression of reflexive saccades in younger and older adults: Age comparisons on an antisaccade task . Memory and Cognition , 27 ( 4 ) : 584 – 591 .
  • Carmi , R. and Itti , L. 2006 . The role of memory in guiding attention during natural vision . Journal of Vision , 6 ( 9 ) : 898 – 914 .
  • Chernyak , D. A. and Stark , L. W. 2001 . Top-down guided eye movements . IEEE Transactions on Systems Man and Cybernetics: Part B Cybernetics , 31 ( 4 ) : 514 – 522 .
  • Cooper , R. P. 2010 . Editor's introduction: Cognitive control . Topics in Cognitive Science , 2 ( 4 ) : 598 – 613 .
  • Einhauser , W. , Rutishauser , U. and Koch , C. 2008 . Task-demands can immediately reverse the effects of sensory-driven saliency in complex visual stimuli . Journal of Vision , 8 ( 2 ) : 1 – 19 .
  • Everling , S. and Fischer , B. 1998 . The antisaccade: A review of basic research and clinical studies . Neuropsychologica , 36 ( 9 ) : 885 – 899 .
  • Findlay , J. M. 1982 . Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements . Vision Research , 22 : 1033 – 1045 .
  • Findlay , J. M. 1997 . Saccade target selection during visual search . Vision Research , 37 ( 5 ) : 617 – 631 .
  • Findlay , M. and Gilchrist , I. D. 2003 . Active vision: The psychology of looking and seeing , Oxford, , UK : Oxford University Press .
  • Fitts , P. M. 1954 . The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement . Journal of Experimental Psychology , 47 ( 6 ) : 381 – 391 .
  • Fitts , P. M. and Posner , M. I. 1967 . Human performance , Belmont, CA : Brooks Cole .
  • Foulsham , T. and Underwood , G. 2007 . How does the purpose of inspection influence the potency of visual salience in scene perception? . Perception , 36 : 1123 – 1138 .
  • Franconeri , S. L. and Simons , D. J. 2003 . Moving and looming stimuli capture attention . Perception and Psychophysics , 65 ( 7 ) : 999 – 1010 .
  • Franconeri , S. L. , Simons , D. J. and Junge , J. A. 2004 . Searching for stimulus-driven shifts of attention . Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , 11 ( 5 ) : 876 – 881 .
  • Fu , W.-T. and Anderson , J. R. 2006 . From recurrent choice to skill learning: A reinforcement-learning model . Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 135 ( 2 ) : 184 – 206 .
  • Fu , W.-T. and Pirolli , P. 2007 . SNIF-ACT: A cognitive model of user navigation on the World Wide Web . Human–Computer Interaction , 22 ( 2 ) : 355 – 412 .
  • Geisler , W. S. 2003 . “ Ideal observer analysis ” . In The visual neurosciences , Edited by: Chalupa , L. and Werner , J. 825 – 837 . Boston, MA : MIT Press .
  • Gibbon , J. 1977 . Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing . Psychological Review , 84 ( 3 ) : 279 – 325 .
  • Godijn , R. and Kramer , A. F. 2006 . Prosaccades and antisaccades to onsets and color singletons: Evidence that erroneous prosaccades are not reflexive . Experimental Brain Research , 172 ( 4 ) : 439 – 448 .
  • Godijn , R. and Theeuwes , J. 2002 . Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 28 ( 5 ) : 1039 – 1054 .
  • Gray , W. D. 2008 . Cognitive architectures: Choreographing the dance of mental operations with the task environments . Human Factors , 50 ( 3 ) : 497 – 505 .
  • Gray , W. D. and Fu , W.-T. 2004 . Soft constraints in interactive behavior: The case of ignoring perfect knowledge in-the-world for imperfect knowledge in-the-head . Cognitive Science , 28 ( 3 ) : 359 – 382 .
  • Gray , W. D. , Schoelles , M. J. and Myers , C. W. 2003 . Meeting Newell's other challenge: Cognitive architectures as the basis for cognitive engineering . Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 26 ( 5 ) : 609 – 610 .
  • Gray , W. D. , Sims , C. R. , Fu , W.-T. and Schoelles , M. 2006 . The soft constraints hypothesis: A rational analysis approach to resource allocation for interactive behavior . Psychological Review , 113 ( 3 ) : 461 – 482 .
  • Hallett , P. E. 1978 . Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions . Vision Research , 18 ( 10 ) : 1279 – 1296 .
  • Hallett , P. E. and Adams , B. D. 1980 . The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task . Vision Research , 20 ( 4 ) : 329 – 339 .
  • Hayhoe , M. M. 2000 . Vision using routines: A functional account of vision . Visual Cognition , 7 ( 1–3 ) : 43 – 64 .
  • Hayhoe , M. M. and Ballard , D. H. 2005 . Eye movements in natural behavior . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 9 ( 4 ) : 187 – 194 .
  • Hayhoe , M. M. , Shrivastava , A. , Mruczek , R. and Pelz , J. B. 2003 . Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task . Journal of Vision , 3 ( 1 ) : 49 – 63 .
  • Henderson , J. M. 2007 . Regarding scenes . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 16 ( 4 ) : 219 – 222 .
  • Horowitz , T. S. and Wolfe , J. M. 2001 . Search for multiple targets: Remember the targets, forget the search . Perception and Psychophysics , 63 : 272 – 285 .
  • Horowitz , T. S. and Wolfe , J. M. 2003 . Memory for rejected distractors in visual search? . Visual Cognition , 10 ( 3 ) : 257 – 298 .
  • Itti , L. 2006 . Quantitative modelling of perceptual salience at human eye position . Visual Cognition , 14 ( 4/5/6/7/8 ) : 959 – 984 .
  • Itti , L. and Koch , C. 2001 . Computational modelling of visual attention . Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 2 : 194 – 204 .
  • Itti , L. , Koch , C. and Niebur , E. 1998 . A model of saliency-based visual attention for rapid scene analysis . IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence , 20 ( 11 ) : 1254 – 1259 .
  • Jacobs , R. A. and Kruschke , J. K. 2011 . Bayesian learning theory applied to human cognition . Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science , 2 ( 1 ) : 8 – 21 .
  • Karsh , R. , & Breitenbach , F. W. . 1983 . Looking at looking: Amorphous fixation measure . Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  • Keim , D. A. , Robertson , G. G. , Thomas , J. J. and van Wijk , J. J. 2006 . Guest editorial: Special section on visual analytics . IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics , 12 ( 6 ) : 1361 – 1362 .
  • Kowler , E. 1990 . “ The role of visual and cognitive processes in the control of eye movement ” . In Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes , Edited by: Kowler , E. 1 – 70 . New York, NY : Elsevier .
  • Kramer , A. F. , Hahn , S. , Irwin , D. E. and Theeuwes , J. 2000 . Age differences in the control of looking behavior: Do you know where your eyes have been? . Psychological Science , 11 ( 3 ) : 210 – 217 .
  • Körding , K. 2007 . Decision theory: What “should” the nervous system do? . Science , 318 : 606 – 610 .
  • Lamy , D. and Egeth , H. E. 2003 . Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 29 ( 5 ) : 1003 – 1020 .
  • Land , M. F. , Furneaux , S. M. and Gilchrist , I. D. 2002 . The organization of visually mediated actions in a subject without eye movements . Neurocase , 8 ( 1–2 ) : 80 – 87 .
  • Land , M. F. and Lee , D. N. 1994 . Where we look when we steer . Nature , 369 : 742 – 744 .
  • Land , M. F. and McLeod , P. 2000 . From eye movements to actions: How batsmen hit the ball . Nature Neuroscience , 3 : 1340 – 1345 .
  • Land , M. F. and Tatler , B. W. 2001 . Steering with the head: The visual strategy of a racing driver . Current Biology , 11 : 1215 – 1220 .
  • Larson , G. E. and Perry , Z. A. 1999 . Visual capture and human error . Applied Cognitive Psychology , 13 ( 3 ) : 227 – 236 .
  • Lee , B. , Parr , C. S. , Plaisant , C. , Bederson , B. B. , Veksler , V. D. Gray , W. D. 2006 . Visualizing networks with enhanced tree layouts: Can interaction tame complexity? . IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics , 12 ( 6 ) : 1414 – 1426 .
  • Macmillan , N. A. and Creelman , C. D. 1991 . Detection theory: A user's guide , Cambridge, , UK : Cambridge University Press .
  • Maloney , L. T. 2003 . Statistical decision theory and evolution . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 7 ( 11 ) : 473 – 475 .
  • Maloney , L. T. , Trommershauser , J. and Land , M. F. 2007 . “ Questions without words: A comparison between decision making under risk and movement planning under risk ” . In Integrated models of cognitive systems , Edited by: Gray , W. D. 297 – 313 . Oxford, , UK : Oxford University Press .
  • McCarley , J. S. , Kramer , A. F. , Wickens , C. D. , Vidoni , E. D. and Boot , W. R. 2004 . Visual skills in airport-security screening . Psychological Science , 15 ( 5 ) : 302 – 306 .
  • Mitchell , J. P. , Macrae , C. N. and Gilchrist , I. D. 2002 . Working memory and the suppression of reflexive saccades . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 14 ( 1 ) : 95 – 103 .
  • Mulckhuyse , M. and Theeuwes , J. 2010 . Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature . Acta Psychologica , 134 ( 3 ) : 299 – 309 .
  • Munneke , J. , Van der Stigchel , S. and Theeuwes , J. 2008 . Cueing the location of a distractor: An inhibitory mechanism of spatial attention? . Acta Psychologica , 129 : 101 – 107 .
  • Myers , C. W. , & Gray , W. D. 2010 . Visual scan adaptation during repeated visual search . Journal of Vision , 10 4 , 1 – 14 .
  • Müller , H. J. and Krummenacher , J. 2006 . Locus of dimension weighting: Preattentive or postselective? . Visual Cognition , 14 ( 4–8 ) : 490 – 513 .
  • Najemnik , J. and Geisler , W. S. 2005 . Optimal eye movement strategies in visual search . Nature , 434 : 387 – 391 .
  • Najemnik , J. and Geisler , W. S. 2008 . Eye movement statistics in humans are consistent with an optimal search strategy . Journal of Vision , 8 ( 3 ) : 1 – 14 .
  • Nieuwenhuis , S. , Ridderinkhof , K. R. , de Jong , R. , Kok , A. and van der Molen , M. W. 2000 . Inhibitory inefficiency and failures of intention activation: Age-related decline in the control of saccadic eye movements . Psychology and Aging , 15 ( 4 ) : 635 – 647 .
  • North , D. W. 1968 . A tutorial introduction to decision theory . IEEE Transactions on Systems Science and Cybernetics , 4 ( 3 ) : 200 – 210 .
  • Peterson , M. S. , Kramer , A. F. , Wang , R. F. , Irwin , D. E. and McCarley , J. S. 2001 . Visual search has memory . Psychological Science , 12 : 287 – 292 .
  • Pomplun , M. , Reingold , E. M. and Shen , J. Y. 2003 . Area activation: A computational model of saccadic selectivity in visual search . Cognitive Science , 27 ( 2 ) : 299 – 312 .
  • Ratcliff , R. 1979 . Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics . Psychological Bulletin , 56 : 446 – 461 .
  • Rayner , K. 1998 . Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research . Psychological Bulletin , 124 ( 3 ) : 372 – 422 .
  • Rosenholtz , R. 1999 . A simple saliency model predicts a number of motion popout phenomena . Vision Research , 39 : 3157 – 3163 .
  • Rosenholtz , R. 2001 . Search asymmetries? What search asymmetries? . Perception and Psychophysics , 63 ( 3 ) : 476 – 489 .
  • Streeter , M. J. , & Smith , S. F . 2006 . An asymptotically optimal algorithm for the max k-armed bandit problem . In Proceedings of the twenty-first national conference on artificial intelligence pp. 135 – 142 . Boston, MA : AAAI Press
  • Sutton , R. S. and Barto , A. G. 1998 . Reinforcement learning: An introduction , Cambridge, MA : MIT Press .
  • Teo , L. and John , B. E. 2010 . “ Three steps in the evolution of a goal-directed exploration model: Effects of information scent and go-back utility on successful exploration ” . In Tenth international conference on cognitive modeling (ICCM-10) , Edited by: Salvucci , D. D. and Gunzelmann , G. 149 – 154 . Philadelphia, PA : Drexel University .
  • Theeuwes , J. 2004 . Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture . Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , 11 ( 1 ) : 65 – 70 .
  • Theeuwes , J. , Kramer , A. F. , Hahn , S. and Irwin , D. E. 1998 . Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects . Psychological Science , 9 ( 5 ) : 379 – 385 .
  • Thomas , J. J. , & Cook , K. A. 2005 . Illuminating the path: The research and development agenda for visual analytics . Washington, DC : IEEE Press .
  • Thrun , S. 1992 . “ The role of exploration in learning control ” . In Handbook of intelligent control: Neural, fuzzy and adaptive approaches , Edited by: White , D. A. and Sofge , D. A. 527 – 559 . New York, NY : Van Nostrand Reinhold .
  • Treisman , A. and Gelade , G. 1980 . A feature integration theory of attention . Cognitive Psychology , 12 : 97 – 136 .
  • Trommershauser , J. , Maloney , L. T. and Landy , M. S. 2003a . Statistical decision theory and the selection of rapid, goal-directed movements . Journal of Optical Society of America , 20A ( 7 ) : 1419 – 1433 .
  • Trommershauser , J. , Maloney , L. T. and Landy , M. S. 2003b . Statistical decision theory and trade-offs in the control of motor response . Spatial Vision , 16 ( 3–4 ) : 255 – 275 .
  • Wolfe , J. M. 1994 . Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search . Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , 1 ( 2 ) : 202 – 238 .
  • Wolfe , J. M. 1998 . What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search? . Psychological Science , 9 ( 1 ) : 33 – 39 .
  • Wolfe , J. M. , Alvarez , G. A. and Horowitz , T. S. 2000 . Attention is fast but volition is slow . Nature , 406 ( 6797 ) : 691
  • Wolfe , J. M. , Horowitz , T. S. , van Wert , M. J. , Kenner , N. M. , Place , S. S. and Kibbi , N. 2007 . Low target prevalence is a stubborn source of errors in visual search tasks . Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 136 ( 4 ) : 623 – 638 .
  • Yantis , S. and Jonides , J. 1990 . Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 16 : 121 – 134 .
  • Yarbus , A. L. 1967 . Eye movements and vision , New York, NY : Plenum Press .

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.