3,359
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Perceptual span, visual span, and visual attention span: Three potential ways to quantify limits on visual processing during reading

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 412-429 | Received 22 Aug 2017, Accepted 26 Apr 2018, Published online: 11 May 2018

References

  • Adelman, J. S., Marquis, S. J., & Sabatos de Vito, S. G. (2010). Letters in words are read simultaneously, not left-to-right. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1799–1801. doi: 10.1177/0956797610387442
  • Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multi-trace memory model of polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105, 678–723. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.4.678-723
  • Apel, J. K., Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (2012). Targeting regressions: Do readers pay attention to the left? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(6), 1108–1113. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0291-1
  • Ashby, J., Yang, J., Evans, K. H., & Rayner, K. (2012). Eye movements and the perceptual span in silent and oral reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(4), 634–640. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0277-0
  • Averbach, E., & Sperling, G. (1968). Short term storage of information in vision. In R. N. Haber (Ed.), Contemporary theory and research in visual perception (pp. 196–211). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  • Awadh, F. H. R., Phénix, T., Antzaka, A., Lallier, M., Carreiras, M., & Valdois, S. (2016). Cross-language modulation of visual attention span: An Arabic-French-Spanish comparison in skilled adult readers. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 307. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4779959/ doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00307
  • Baron, J., & Thurston, I. (1973). An analysis of the word-superiority effect. Cognitive Psychology, 4(2), 207–228. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90012-1
  • Bélanger, N. N., Slattery, T. J., Mayberry, R. I., & Rayner, K. (2012). Skilled deaf readers have an enhanced perceptual span in reading. Psychological Science, 23, 816–823. doi: 10.1177/0956797611435130
  • Blanchard, H. E., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1989). The acquisition of parafoveal word information in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 85–94. doi: 10.3758/BF03208078
  • Bosse, M. L., Chaves, N., Largy, P., & Valdois, S. (2015). Orthographic learning during reading: The role of whole-word visual processing. Journal of Research in Reading, 38(2), 141–158. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01551.x
  • Bosse, M.-L., Kandel, S., Prado, C., & Valdois, S. (2014). Does visual attention span relate to eye movements during reading and copying? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(1), 81–85. doi: 10.1177/0165025413509046
  • Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104(2), 198–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.009
  • Bosse, M.-L., & Valdois, S. (2009). Influence of the visual attention span on child reading performance: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(2), 230–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2008.01387.x
  • Bouma, H. (1970). Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature, 226(241), 177–178. doi: 10.1038/226177a0
  • Bullimore, M. A., & Bailey, I. L. (1995). Reading and eye movements in age-related maculopathy. Optometry and Vision Science, 72, 125–138. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199502000-00011
  • Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological Review, 97, 523–547. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.523
  • Bundesen, C. (1998). Visual selective attention: Outlines of a choice model, a race model and a computational theory. Visual Cognition, 5, 287–309. doi: 10.1080/713756774
  • Chen, Y., Fu, S., Iversen, S. D., Smith, S. M., & Matthews, P. M. (2002). Testing for dual brain processing routes in reading: A direct contrast of Chinese character and pinyin reading using fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7), 1088–1098. doi: 10.1162/089892902320474535
  • Cheong, A. M., Legge, G. E., Lawrence, M. G., Cheung, S. H., & Ruff, M. A. (2008). Relationship between visual span and reading performance in age-related macular degeneration. Vision Research, 48(4), 577–588. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.11.022
  • Choi, W., Lowder, M. W., Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (2015). Individual differences in the perceptual span during reading: Evidence from the moving window technique. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 77(7), 2463–2475. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0942-1
  • Chung, H. K., Liu, J. Y., & Hsiao, J. H. (2017). How does reading direction modulate perceptual asymmetry effects? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(8), 1559–1574. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1193549
  • Chung, S. T. L., Legge, G. E., & Cheung, S.-H. (2004). Letter-recongition and reading speed in peripheral vision benefit from perceptual learning. Vision Research, 44, 695–709. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.09.028
  • Crossland, M. D., & Rubin, G. S. (2006). Eye movements and reading in macular disease: Further support for the shrinking perceptual span hypothesis. Vision Research, 46(4), 590–597. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.05.015
  • Dubois, M., Kyllingsbaek, S., Prado, C., Musca, S. C., Peiffer, E., Lassus-Sangosse, D., & Valdois, S. (2010). Fractionating the multi-character processing deficit in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Cortex, 46, 717–738. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.002
  • Ellis, A. W., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Split fovea theory and the role of the two cerebral hemispheres in reading: A review of the evidence. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 353–365. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.021
  • Engbert, R., Longtin, A., & Kliegl, R. (2002). A dynamic model of saccade generation in reading based on spatially distributed lexical processing. Vision Research, 42(5), 621–636. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00301-7
  • Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Swift: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112, 777–813. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777
  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211
  • Flom, M. C., Weymouth, F. W., & Kahneman, D. (1963). Visual resolution and contour interaction. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 53, 1026–1032. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.53.001026
  • Frey, A. (2016). Eye movements in children during reading: A review. In Actes du Symposium International sur la Litéracie à l’Ecole/ International symposium for educational Literacy (SILE/ ISEL) 2015 (pp. 69–90). Sherbrooke: Les Editions de l’Université de Sherbrooke.
  • Frey, A., Ionescu, G., Lemaire, B., Lopez-Orozco, F., Baccino, T., & Guérin-Dugué, A. (2013). Decision-making in information seeking on texts: En eye-fixation-related potentials investigation. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. Research Topic on «Eye movement-related brain activity during perceptual and cognitive processing», 7, 39. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3743136/ doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00039
  • Germano, G. D., Reilhac, C., Capellini, S. A., & Valdois, S. (2014). The phonological and visual basis of developmental dyslexia in Brazilian Portuguese reading children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1169. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4196516/ doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01169
  • Gleason, J. B. (2005). The development of language (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc.
  • Goswami, U. (2015). Sensory theories of developmental dyslexia: Three challenges for research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(1), 43–54. doi: 10.1038/nrn3836
  • Grainger, J., Dufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2016). A vision of reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(3), 171–179. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.008
  • He, Y., Scholz, J. M., Gage, R., Kallie, C. S., Liu, T., & Legge, G. E. (2015). Comparing the visual spans for faces and letters. Journal of Vision, 11(8), 1–13.
  • Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (1990). Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: Implications for attention and eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 417–429.
  • Huey, E. B. (1908). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon Peretz, J. (2002). The characteristics of the Arabic orthography slow its cognitive processing. Neuropsychologia, 16(3), 322–326. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.322
  • Inhoff, A. W., Pollatsek, A., Posner, M. I., & Rayner, K. (1989). Covert attention and eye movements during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 41(A), 63–89. doi: 10.1080/14640748908402353
  • Jordan, T. R., Almabruk, A. A., Gadalla, E. A., McGowan, V. A., White, S. J., Abedipour, L., & Paterson, K. B. (2014). Reading direction and the central perceptual span: Evidence from Arabic and English. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 505–511. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0510-4
  • Jordan, T. R., Dixon, J., McGowan, V. A., Kurtev, S., & Paterson, K. B. (2016). Effects of spatial frequencies on word identification by fast and slow readers: Evidence from eye movements. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1433. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5039934/ doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01433
  • Jordan, T. R., McGowan, V. A., Kurtev, S., & Paterson, K. B. (2016). A further look at postview effects in reading: An eye-movements study of influences from the left of fixation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(2), 296–307.
  • Jordan, T. R., McGowan, V. A., & Paterson, K. B. (2012). Reading with a filtered fovea: The influence of visual quality at the point of fixation during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 1078–1084. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0307-x
  • Jordan, T. R., & Paterson, K. B. (2010). Where is the evidence for split fovea processing in word recognition? Neuropsychologia, 48, 2782–2783. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.029
  • Kilho, S., Chung, S. T. L., & Bosco, S. T. (2017). Crowding, visual awareness, and their respective neural loci. Journal of Vision, 17(5), 1–18. doi: 10.1167/17.5.1
  • Kliegl, R., Wei, P., Dambacher, M., Yan, M., & Zhou, X. (2011). Experimental effects and individual differences in linear mixed models: Estimating the relationship between spatial, object, and attraction effects in visual attention. Frontiers in Psychology: Quantitative Psychology and Measurement, 1, 238.
  • Kwon, M., Legge, G. E., & Dubbels, B. R. (2007). Developmental changes in the visual span for reading. Vision Research, 47, 2889–2900. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.002
  • Kwon, M., Liu, R., Patel, B. N., & Girkin, C. (2017). Slow reading in Glaucoma: Is it due to the shrinking visual span in central vision? Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 58(13), 5810–5818. doi: 10.1167/iovs.17-22560
  • Laberge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 293–323. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2
  • Lallier, M., Abu Mallouh, R., Mohammed, A. M., Khalifa, B., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2018). Does the visual attention span play a role in reading in Arabic? Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(2), 181–190. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2017.1421958
  • Lallier, M., Acha, J., & Carreiras, M. (2016). Cross-linguistic interactions influence reading development in bilinguals: A comparison between early balanced French-Basque and Spanish-Basque bilingual children. Developmental Science, 19(1), 76–89. doi: 10.1111/desc.12290
  • Lallier, M., Valdois, S., Lassus-Sangosse, D., Prado, C., & Kandel, S. (2014). Impact of orthographic transparency on typical and atypical reading development: Evidence in French-Spanish bilingual children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(5), 1177–1190. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.01.021
  • Lassus-Sangosse, D., N’Guyen-Morel, M. A., & Valdois, S. (2008). Sequential or simultaneous visual processing deficit in developmental dyslexia? Vision Research, 48, 979–988. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.01.025
  • Legge, G. E., Ahn, S. J., Klitz, T. S., & Luebker, A. (1997). Psychophysics of reading. XVI. The visual span in normal and low vision. Vision Research, 37, 1999–2010. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00017-5
  • Legge, G. E., & Bigelow, C. A. (2012). Does print size matter for reading? A review of findings from vision science and typography. Journal of Vision, 11(5), 1–22.
  • Legge, G. E., Cheung, S. H., Yu, D., Chung, S. T. L., Lee, H.-W., & Owens, D. P. (2007). The case for the visual span as a sensory bottleneck in reading. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–15. doi: 10.1167/7.2.9
  • Legge, G. E., Hooven, T. A., Klitz, T. S., Mansfield, J. S., & Tjan, B. S. (2002). Mr. Chips 2002: New insights from an ideal-observer model of reading. Vision Research, 42, 2219–2234. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00131-1
  • Legge, G. E., Klitz, T. S., & Tjan, B. S. (1997). Mr. Chips: An ideal-observer model of reading. Psychological Review, 104, 524–553. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.3.524
  • Legge, G. E., Mansfield, J. S., & Chung, S. T. L. (2001). Psychophysics of reading. XX. Linking letter recognition to reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Vision Research, 41, 725–734. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00295-9
  • Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding. Current Biology, 21(18), 678–679. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.07.025
  • Levi, D. M., & Klein, S. A. (1985). Vernier acuity, crowding and amblyopia. Vision Research, 25, 979–991. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90208-1
  • Liu, D. (2014). Visual-spatial attention and its relationship with reading. In K. K. H. Chung, K. C. P. Yuen, & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Understanding developmental disorders of auditory processing, language and literacy across languages, international perspectives (pp. 169–188). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
  • Lobier, M., Dubois, M., & Valdois, S. (2013). The role of visual processing speed in reading speed development. PLoS ONE, 8, 4. Retrieved from http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0058097
  • Lobier, M., Peyrin, C., Le Bas, J. F., & Valdois, S. (2012). Pre-orthographic character string processing and parietal cortex: A role for visual attention in reading? Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2195–2204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.023
  • Lobier, M., Peyrin, C., Pichat, C., Le Bas, J. F., & Valdois, S. (2014). Visual processing of multiple elements in the dyslexic brain: Evidence for a double dysfunction of parietal and occipito-temporal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 479. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4083222/ doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00479
  • Lobier, M., & Valdois, S. (2015). Visual attention deficits in developmental dyslexia cannot be ascribed solely to poor reading experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 225–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3836-c1
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375–405. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375
  • McConkie, G. W., & Rayner, K. (1975). The span of the effective stimulus during a fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 578–586. doi: 10.3758/BF03203972
  • McConkie, G. W., & Rayner, K. (1976). Asymmetry of the perceptual span in reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 8, 365–368. doi: 10.3758/BF03335168
  • Miellet, S., O’Donnell, P. J., & Sereno, S. C. (2009). Parafoveal magnification: Visual acuity does not modulate the perceptual span in reading. Psychological Science, 20, 721–728. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02364.x
  • Miellet, S., & Sparrow, L. (2004). Phonological codes are assembled before word fixation: Evidence from boundary paradigm in sentence reading. Brain and Language, 90(1), 299–310. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00442-5
  • Nation, K., & Castles, A. (2017). Putting the learning in orthographic learning. In K. Cain, D. Compton, & R. Parilla (Eds.), Theories of reading development (pp. 148–168). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Nazir, T. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Jacobs, A. M. (1991). On words and their letters. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 171–174. doi: 10.3758/BF03335227
  • Ojanpää, H., Näsänen, R., & Kojo, I. (2002). Eye movements in the visual search of word lists. Vision Research, 42, 1499–1512. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00077-9
  • O’Regan, J. K. (1990). Eye movements and reading. In E. Kowler (Ed.), Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes (pp. 395–453). New York: Elsevier.
  • O’Regan, J. K. (1991). Understanding visual search and reading using the concept of stimulus “grain”. IPO Annual Progress Reports, 26, 96–108.
  • Paterson, K. B., McGowan, V. A., White, S. J., Malik, S., Abedipour, L., & Jordan, T. R. (2014). Reading direction and the central perceptual span in Urdu and English. PLoS ONE, 9(2). Retrieved from https://scirp.org/reference/ReferencesPapers.aspx?ReferenceID=1334371 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088358
  • Pelli, D. G. (2009). Towards an easier way to measure the visual span. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 1002–1002. doi: 10.1167/9.8.1002
  • Perea, M., Mallouh, R. A., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Are root letters compulsory for lexical access in Semitic languages? The case of masked form-priming in Arabic. Cognition, 132(3), 491–500. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.008
  • Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383. doi: 10.1080/10888430701530730
  • Peyrin, C., Démonet, J. F., N’Guyen-Morel, M. A., Le Bas, J. F., & Valdois, S. (2011). Superior parietal lobule dysfunction in a homogeneous group of dyslexic children with a visual attention span disorder. Brain and Language, 118, 128–138. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.06.005
  • Peyrin, C., Lallier, M., Démonet, J.-F., Pernet, C., Baciu, M., Le Bas, J. F., & Valdois, S. (2012). Neural dissociation of phonological and visual attention span disorders in developmental dyslexia: fMRI evidence from two case reports. Brain and Language, 120, 381–394. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.015
  • Phénix, T., Diard, J., & Valdois, S. (2016). Les modèles computationnels de lecture. In M. Sata, & S. Pinto (Eds.), Traité de neurolinguistique (pp. 167–182). Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium: De Boeck supérieur.
  • Pollatsek, A., Bolozky, S., Well, A. D., & Rayner, K. (1981). Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli readers. Brain and Language, 14, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(81)90073-0
  • Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K., & Balota, D. A. (1986). Inferences about eye movement control from the perceptual span in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 123–130. doi: 10.3758/BF03208192
  • Pollatsek, A., Reichle, E. D., & Rayner, K. (2006). Tests of the EZ reader model: Exploring the interface between cognition and eye-movement control. Cognitive Psychology, 52(1), 1–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.06.001
  • Pomplun, M., Reingold, E. M., & Shen, J. (2001). Investigating the visual span in comparative search: The effects of task difficulty and divided attention. Cognition, 81, 57–67. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00123-8
  • Prado, C., Dubois, M., & Valdois, S. (2007). The eye movements of dyslexic children during reading and visual search: Impact of the visual attention span. Vision Research, 47(19), 2521–2530. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.06.001
  • Rayner, K. (1977). Visual attention in reading: Eye movements reflect cognitive processes. Memory & Cognition, 4, 443–448. doi: 10.3758/BF03197383
  • Rayner, K. (1981). Eye movements and the perceptual span in reading. In F. J. Pirozzolo, & M. C. Wittroch (Eds.), Neuropsychological and cognitive processes in reading (pp. 145–165). New York: Academic Press.
  • Rayner, K. (1984). Visual selection in reading, picture perception, and visual search: A tutorial review. In H. Bouma, & D. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. 10, pp. 67–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Rayner, K. (1986). Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41(2), 211–236. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(86)90037-8
  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372
  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements in reading: Models and data. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(5), 1–10.
  • Rayner, K. (2014). The gaze-contingent moving window in reading: Development and review. Visual Cognition, 22, 242–258. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2013.879084
  • Rayner, K., Abbott, M. J., & Plummer, P. (2015). Individual differences in perceptual processing and eye movements in reading. In Handbook of individual differences in reading: Text and context. New York, NY: Informa UK Limited.
  • Rayner, K., & Bertera, J. H. (1979). Reading without a fovea. Science, 206, 468–469. doi: 10.1126/science.504987
  • Rayner, K., & Inhoff, A. W. (1981). Control of eye movements during reading. In B. L. Zuber (Ed.), Models of oculomotor behavior and control. West Palm Beach, FL: CRC Press.
  • Rayner, K., Liversedge, S. P., White, S. J., & Vergilino-Perez, D. (2003). Reading disappearing text: Cognitive control of eye movements. Psychological Science, 14, 385–388. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.24483
  • Rayner, K., Slattery, T. J., & Bélanger, N. (2010). Eye movements, the perceptual span, and reading speed. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(6), 834–839. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.6.834
  • Rayner, K., Well, A. D., Pollatsek, A., & Bertera, J. H. (1982). The availability of useful information to the right of fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 537–550. doi: 10.3758/BF03204186
  • Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2006). E–Z reader: A cognitive-control, serial-attention model of eye-movement behavior during reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7(1), 4–22. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.07.002
  • Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E-Z reader model of eye movement control in reading: Comparison to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 445–476. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X03000104
  • Risse, S. (2014). Effects of visual span on reading speed and parafoveal processing in eye movements during sentence reading. Journal of Vision, 14(8), 1–13. doi: 10.1167/14.8.11
  • Sargiani, R., Maluf, M. R., & Bosse, M.-L. (2015). O papel da amplitude visuoatencional e da consciência fonêmica na aprendizagem da leitura (The role of visual attention span and phonemic awareness in learning to read). Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 28(3), 593–602.
  • Schad, D. J., & Engbert, R. (2012). The zoom lens of attention: Simulating shuffled versus normal text reading using the SWIFT model. Visual Cognition, 20, 391–421. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2012.670143
  • Schotter, E. R., Angele, B., & Rayner, K. (2012). Parafoveal processing in reading. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 74, 5–35. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0219-2
  • Shin, K., Chung, S. T. L., & Tjan, B. S. (2017). Crowding, visual awareness, and their respective neural loci. Journal of Vision, 17(5), 18. doi: 10.1167/17.5.18
  • Silva, C., Faísca, L., Ingvar, M., Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2012). Literacy: Exploring working memory systems. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 369–377. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.645017
  • Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11), 1–29. doi: 10.1037/h0093759
  • Starrfelt, R., Habekost, T., & Leff, A. P. (2009). Too little, too late: Reduced visual span and speed characterize pure alexia. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2880–2890. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp059
  • Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., Zorman, M., David, D., & Pellat, J. (2003). Phonological and visual processing deficits can dissociate in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Reading and Writing, 16, 541–572. doi: 10.1023/A:1025501406971
  • Valdois, S., Guinet, E., & Embs, J.-L. (2014). EVADYS, logiciel d’évaluation de l’empan visuo-attentionnel chez l’enfant dyslexique (Software for the assessment of the visual attention span of dyslexic children). Isbergues: Ortho-Editions.
  • Valdois, S., Peyrin, C., Lassus-Sangosse, D., Lallier, M., Démonet, J.-F., & Kandel, S. (2014). Dyslexia in a French-Spanish bilingual girl: Behavioural and neural modulations following a visual attention span intervention. Cortex, 53, 120–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.11.006
  • van den Boer, M., de Jong, P. F., & Haentjens-van Meeteren, M. (2013). Modeling the length effect: Specifying the relation with visual and phonological correlates of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 243–256. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2012.683222
  • van den Boer, M., van Bergen, E., & de Jong, P. F. (2014). Underlying skills of oral and silent reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 138–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.07.008
  • van den Boer, M., van Bergen, E., & de Jong, P. F. (2015). The specific relation of visual attention span with reading and spelling in Dutch. Learning and Individual Differences, 39, 141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.03.017
  • Veldre, A., & Andrews, S. (2014). Lexical quality and eye movements: Individual differences in the perceptual span of skilled adult readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 703–727. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.826258
  • Wei, C.-C., & Ma, M.-Y. (2016). Influences of visual attention and reading time on children and adults. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 33(2), 97–108. doi: 10.1080/10573569.2015.1092100
  • Whitford, V., & Titone, D. (2016). Eye movements and the perceptual span during first- and second-language sentence reading in bilingual older adults. Psychology and Aging, 31(1), 58–70. doi: 10.1037/a0039971
  • Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 221–243. doi: 10.3758/BF03196158
  • Whitney, D., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on conscious perception and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.005
  • Woodworth, R. S. (1938). Experimental psychology. New York: Henry Holt & Co.
  • Yan, M., Zhou, W., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2015). Perceptual span depends on font size during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(1), 209–219.
  • Yu, D., Cheung, S. H., Legge, G. E., & Chung, S. T. L. (2007). Effect of letter spacing on visual span and reading speed. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–10. doi: 10.1167/7.2.2
  • Yu, D., Legge, G. E., Wagoner, G., & Chung, S. T. L. (2014). Sensory factors limiting horizontal and vertical visual span for letter recognition. Journal of Vision, 9(8), 820. doi: 10.1167/9.8.820
  • Yu, D., Park, H., Gerold, D., & Legge, G. E. (2010). Comparing reading speed for horizontal and vertical English text. Journal of Vision, 10(2), 1–17. doi: 10.1167/10.2.21
  • Zhao, J., Liu, M., Liu, H., & Huang, C. (2018). The visual attention span deficit in Chinese children with reading fluency difficulty. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 73, 76–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.12.017
  • Zoubrinetzky, R., Bielle, F., & Valdois, S. (2014). New insights on developmental dyslexia subtypes: Heterogeneity of mixed reading profiles. PloS ONE, 9(6). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099337

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.