158
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Spatial layout extrapolation in aging: underlying cognitive and executive mechanisms

, &
Pages 668-686 | Received 18 Jun 2018, Accepted 27 May 2019, Published online: 12 Jul 2019

References

  • Arndt, J. (2010). The role of memory activation in creating false memories of encoding context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 36(1), 66–79.
  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423.
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.
  • Balota, D. A., Cortese, J. M., Duchek, J. M., Adams, D., Roediger III, H. L., McDermott, K. B., & Yerys, B. E. (1999). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia in Alzheimer’s type. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 361–384.
  • Bar, M. (2004). Visual objects in context. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 617–629.
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.
  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 164–169.
  • Butler, K. M., McDaniel, M. A., Dornburg, C. C., Price, A. L., & Roediger III, H. L. (2004). Age differences in veridical and false recall are not inevitable: The role of frontal lobe function. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 921–925.
  • Chadwick, M. J., Mullally, S. L., & Maguire, E. A. (2013). The hippocampus extrapolates beyond the view in scenes: An fMRI study of boundary extension. Cortex, 49, 2067–2079.
  • Chan, J. C., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). The testing effect in recognition memory: A dual process account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 431–437.
  • Chan, R.C.K., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., & Chen E.Y.H. (2008). Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 201–216.
  • Chapman, P., Ropar, D., Mitchell, P., & Ackroyd, K. (2005). Understanding boundary extension: Normalisation and extension errors in picture memory among adults and boys with and without Asperger’s syndrome. Visual Cognition, 12, 1265–1290.
  • Clément, J. P. (1997). Instruments d’évaluation en psychiatrie du sujet âgé. In J. M. Léger, J. P. Clément et J. Wertheimer (Eds.), Psychiatrie du sujet âgé (pp. 570–588). Paris: Flammarion.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge Academic.
  • Cohen, J. D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). Psyscope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 25, 257–271.
  • Collette, F., & Salmon, E. (2014). Les modifications du fonctionnement exécutif dans le vieillissement normal [Executive dysfunction in normal aging]. Psychologie française, 59, 41–58.
  • Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain (Doctoral dissertation, McGill University). Dissertation Abstracts International 34: 819–B.
  • Corson, Y., & Verrier, N. (2013). Les faux souvenirs. Bruxelles: De Boeck.
  • Critchley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. New York: Hafner.
  • Czigler, I., Intraub, H., & Stefanics, G. (2013). Prediction beyond the borders: ERP indices of boundary extension-related error. PLoS ONE, 8, e74245.
  • De Beni, R., Palladino, P., Borella, E., & Lo Presti, S. (2003). Reading comprehension and aging: Does an age-related difference necessarily mean impairment? Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 15, 67–76.
  • Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22.
  • De Frias, C. M., Dixon, R. A., & Strauss, E. (2006). Structure of four executive functioning tests in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology, 20, 206–214.
  • De Frias, C. M., Dixon, R. A., & Strauss, E. (2009). Characterizing executive functioning in older special populations: From cognitively elite to cognitively impaired. Neuropsychology, 23, 778–791.
  • Dehon, H. (2006). Variations in processing resources and resistance to false memories in younger and older adults. Memory, 14, 692–711.
  • Dennis, N. A., Kim, H., & Cabeza, R. (2007). Effects of aging on true and false memory formation: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3157–3166.
  • Devitt, A. L., Tippett, L., Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2016). Autobiographical memory conjunction errors in younger and older adults: Evidence for a role of inhibitory ability. Psychology and Aging, 31, 927–942.
  • Dickinson, C. A., & Intraub, H. (2008). Transsaccadic representation of layout: What is the time course of boundary extension? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 543–555.
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.
  • Fournier, M., & Albaret, J. (2014). Étalonnage des blocs de Corsi sur une population d'enfants scolarisés du CP à la 6e. Développements, 3, 76–82.
  • Gagnier, K. M. (2010). Rethinking boundary extension: The role of source monitoring in scene memory. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses (Accession Order No. [854499379]).
  • Gagnier, K. M., Dickinson, C. A., & Intraub, H. (2013). Fixating picture boundaries does not eliminate boundary extension: Implications for scene representation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2161–2186.
  • Gagnier, K. M., & Intraub, H. (2012). When less is more: Line-drawings lead to greater boundary extension than colour photographs. Visual Cognition, 20, 815–824.
  • Godefroy, O., & GREFEX. (2008). Fonctions exécutives et pathologies neurologiques et psychiatriques: évaluation en pratique clinique. Marseille: Solal.
  • Gold, D., & Arbuckle, T. (1995). A longitudinal study of off-target verbosity. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, P307–P315.
  • Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop colour and word test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago, IL: Stoelting.
  • Gottesman, C. V. (2011). Mental layout extrapolations prime spatial processing of scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception Performance, 37, 382–395.
  • Gottesman, C. V., & Intraub, H. (2002). Surface construal and the mental representation of scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 589–599.
  • Gould, O. N., Trevithick, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1991). Adult age differences in elaborations produced during prose recall. Psychology and Aging, 6, 93–99.
  • Hale, R. G., Brown, J. M., & McDunn, B. A. (2016). Increasing task demand by obstructing object recognition increases boundary extension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1497–1503.
  • Hale, R. G., Brown, J. M., McDunn, B. A., & Siddiqui, A. P. (2015). An influence of extremal edges on boundary extension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 961–966.
  • Hasher, L., Quig, M. B., & May, C. P. (1997). Inhibitory control over no longer relevant information: Adult age differences. Memory and Cognition, 25, 286–295.
  • Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gropher, & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation and performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 663–675). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Holland, C. A., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1990). Autobiographical and text recall in the elderly: An investigation of a processing resource deficit. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42A, 441–470.
  • Intraub, H. (2004). Anticipatory spatial representation of 3D regions explored by sighted observers and a deaf-and-blind observer. Cognition, 94, 19–37.
  • Intraub, H. (2007). Scene perception: The world through a window. In M. A. Peterson, B. Gillam, & H. A. Sedgwick (Eds.), In the mind’s eye: Julian Hochberg on the perception of pictures, films and the world (pp. 454–466). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Intraub, H. (2010). Rethinking scene perception: A multisource model. In B. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 52 (pp. 231–264). Burlington: Academic Press.
  • Intraub, H. (2012). Rethinking visual scene perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3, 117–127.
  • Intraub, H., Bender, R. S., & Mangels, J. A. (1992). Looking at pictures but remembering scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 180–191.
  • Intraub, H., Daniels, K. K., Horowitz, T. S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2008). Looking at scenes while searching for numbers: Dividing attention multiplies space. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1337–1349.
  • Intraub, H., & Dickinson, C. A. (2008). False memory 1/20th of a second later: What the early onset of boundary extension reveals about perception. Psychological Science, 19, 1007–1014.
  • Intraub, H., Gottesman, C. V., Willey, E. V., & Zuk, I. J. (1996). Boundary extension for briefly glimpsed photographs: Do common perceptual processes result in unexpected memory distortions? Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 118–134.
  • Intraub, H., Morelli, F., & Gagnier, K. M. (2015). Visual, haptic and bimodal scene perception: Evidence for a unitary representation. Cognition, 138, 132–147.
  • Intraub, H., & Richardson, M. (1989). Wide-angle memories of close-up scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 179–187.
  • Jacoby, L. L., & Rhodes, M. G. (2006). False remembering in the aged. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 49–53.
  • Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.
  • Karpinsky, A. C., & Scullin, M. H. (2009). Suggestibility under pressure: Theory of mind, executive function, and suggestibility in preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 749–763.
  • Kessels, R. P. C., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). The Corsi block-tapping task: Standardisation and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 252–258.
  • Kim, S., Dede, A. J. O., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2015). Memory, scene construction, and the human hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112, 4767–4772. doi:10.1073/ pnas.1503863112
  • Koriat, A., Goldsmith, M., & Pansky, A. (2000). Toward a psychology of memory accuracy. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 481–537.
  • LaVoie, D. J., Willoughby, L., & Faulkner, K. (2006). Frontal lobe dysfunc- tion and false memory susceptibility in older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 32, 1–21.
  • Le Bouëdec, B., Martins, D., Iralde, L., Gauthier, K., & Delaporte, A. (2002). Inhibition et vieillissement. In C. Boujon (Ed.), L’inhibition au carrefour des neurosciences et des sciences de la cognition (pp. 133–157). Marseille: Solal.
  • Lezak, M. D. (1976). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Loftus, E. F. (1997). Creating false memories. Scientific American, 277, 70–75.
  • Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19–31.
  • Lövdén, M. (2003). The episodic memory and inhibition accounts of age-related increases in false memories / A consistency check. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 268–283.
  • Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2008). Aging and memory: A cognitive approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53, 346–353.
  • McDermott, K. B., & Watson, J. M. (2001). The rise and fall of false recall: The impact of presentation duration. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 160–176.
  • McDunn, B. A., Siddiqui, A. P., & Brown, J. M. (2014). Seeking the boundary of boundary extension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 370–375.
  • Ménétrier, E., Didierjean, A., & Robin, F. (2017). Effect of expertise on boundary extension in approach sequences. i-Perception, 8(5), doi: 10.1177/2041669517723652
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
  • Morris, N., & Jones, D. M. (1990). Habituation to irrelevant speech: Effects on a visual short-term memory task. Perception and Psychophysics, 47, 291–297.
  • Mullally, S. L., Intraub, H., & Maguire, E. A. (2012). Attenuated boundary extension produces a paradoxical memory advantage in amnesic patients. Current Biology, 22, 261–268.
  • Multhaup, K. S., Munger, M. P., & Smith, K. C. (2016). Boundary extension is sensitive to hand position in young and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, gbw011. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw011
  • Munger, M. P., & Multhaup, K. S. (2016). No imagination effect on boundary extension. Memory & Cognition, 44, 73–88.
  • Norman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition, 25, 838–848.
  • Park, S., Intraub, H., Yi, D.-J., Widders, D., & Chun, M. M. (2007). Beyond the edges of a view: Boundary extension in human scene-selective visual cortex. Neuron, 54, 335–342.
  • Plancher, G., Guyard, A., Nicolas, S., & Piolino, P. (2009). Mechanisms underlying the production of false memories for famous people’s names in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2527–2536.
  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2003). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 1: General overview. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Marshuetz, C., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Hartley, A., & Koeppe, R. (2002). Neurocognitive ageing of storage and executive processes. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 257–278.
  • Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 1–75.
  • Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in list. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.
  • Roediger, H. L., Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin &Review, 8, 345–407.
  • Roediger III, H. L. (1996). Memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 76–100.
  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age difference in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.
  • Schacter, D. L., & Slotnick, S. D. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of memory distorsion. Neuron, 44, 149–160.
  • Seamon, J. G., Schleger, S. E., Heister, P. M., Landau, S. M., & Blumenthal, B. F. (2002). Misremembering pictured objects: People of all ages demonstrate the boundary extension illusion. The American Journal of Psychology, 115, 151–167.
  • Sorel, O., & Pennequin, V. (2008). Aging of the planning process: The role of executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 66(2), 196–201.
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
  • Taconnat, L., & Lemaire, P. (2014). Fonctions exécutives, vieillissement cognitif et variations stratégiques [Executive functions, cognitive aging and strategic variations]. Psychologie Française, 59, 89–100.
  • Taconnat, L., & Rémy, P. (2006). Les faux souvenirs dans le vieillisse- ment normal: Données empiriques et modèles théoriques [False memories in normal aging: Empirical data and theoretical models]. L’Année Psychologique, 106, 457–486.
  • Van der Linden, M., Meulemans, T., Marczewski, P., & Collette, F. (2000). The relationships between episodic memory, working memory, and executive functions: The contribution of the prefrontal cortex. Psychologica Belgica, 40, 275–297.
  • Verhaeghen, P. (2011). Aging and executive control: Reports of a Demise greatly exaggerated. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 174–180.
  • Verhaeghen, P., & Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control and attention: A review of meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 849–857.
  • Verhaeghen, P., & De Meersman, L. (1998). Aging and the Stroop effect: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 13(1), 120–126.
  • Vieillard, S., & Bougeant, J.-C. (2005). Performances à une tâche de mémoire de travail sous induction émotionnelle négative : influence modulatrice de l'état émotionnel sur les processus exécutifs. L’année Psychologique, 105, 63–104.
  • Wechsler, D. (2000). WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. Paris: Center of Applied Psychology.
  • Yesavage, J. A. (1988). Geriatric depression scale. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24(4), 709–711.
  • Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Li, K. Z. H. (2000). Human memory. In F. I. M. Craik et T. A. Salthouse (Eds.). The handbook of aging and cognition (2ème éd., pp. 293–357). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.