150
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Articles

How does exogenous alerting impact endogenous preparation on a temporal cueing task

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon &
Pages 363-379 | Received 26 Oct 2022, Accepted 26 Jun 2023, Published online: 30 Aug 2023

References

  • Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19, 716–723. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Capizzi, M., Correa, A., & Sanabria, D. (2013). Temporal orienting of attention is interfered by concurrent working memory updating. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 326–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.005
  • Capizzi, M., Sanabria, D., & Correa, Á. (2012). Dissociating controlled from automatic processing in temporal preparation. Cognition, 123(2), 293–302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.005
  • Christie, J., Hilchey, M. D., Mishra, R., & Klein, R. M. (2015). Eye movements are primed toward the center of multiple stimuli even when the interstimulus distances are too large to generate saccade averaging. Experimental Brain Research, 233(5), 1541–1549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4227-7
  • Correa, Á, Lupiáñez, J., Milliken, B., & Tudela, P. (2004). Endogenous temporal orienting of attention In detection and discrimination tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(2), 264–278. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194878
  • Correa, Á, Lupiáñez, J., & Tudela, P. (2005). Attentional preparation based on temporal expectancy modulates processing at the perceptual level. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(2), 328–334. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196380
  • Correa, Á, Sanabria, D., Spence, C., Tudela, P., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Selective temporal attention enhances the temporal resolution of visual perception: Evidence from a temporal order judgment task. Brain Research, 1070(1), 202–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.094
  • Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: The neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7426–7435. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07426.1998
  • Davranche, K., Nazarian, B., Vidal, F., & Coull, J. (2011). Orienting attention in time activates left intraparietal sulcus for both perceptual and motor task goals. Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3318–3330. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00030
  • de la Rosa, M. D., Sanabria, D., Capizzi, M., & Correa, A. (2012). Temporal preparation driven by rhythms is resistant to working memory interference. Frontiers In Psychology, 3, 308. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00308
  • Denison, R. N., Carrasco, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2021). A dynamic normalization model of temporal attention. Nature Human Behaviour, 5, 1674–1685.
  • Denison, R. N., Heeger, D. J., & Carrasco, M. (2017). Attention flexibly trades off across points in time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1142–1151. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1216-1
  • Denison, R. N., Yuval-Greenberg, S., & Carrasco, M. (2019). Directing voluntary temporal attention increases fixational stability. The Journal of Neuroscience, 39(2), 353–363. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1926-18.2018
  • Dixon, P. (2008). Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 447–456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.004
  • Fernández, A., Denison, R. N., & Carrasco, M. (2019). Temporal attention improves perception similarly at foveal and parafoveal locations. Journal Of Vision, 19(1), 12–12. https://doi.org/10.1167/19.1.12
  • Griffin, I. C., Miniussi, C., & Nobre, A. C. (2001). Orienting attention in time. Frontiers in Bioscience, 6(12), D660–D671. https://doi.org/10.2741/Griffin
  • Han, T., & Proctor, R. W. (2022). Effects of a neutral warning signal on spatial two-choice reactions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(4), 754–764. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211037604
  • Kingstone, A. (1992). Combining expectancies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 44(1), 69–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749208401284
  • Klein, R. M., & Lawrence, M. A. (2012). Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 11–28). The Guilford Press.
  • Korolczuk, I., Burle, B., Coull, J. T., & Smigasiewicz, K. (2020). Mechanisms of impulsive responding to temporally predictable events as revealed by electromyography. Neuroscience, 428, 13–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.12.022
  • Lawrence, M. A., & Klein, R. M. (2013). Isolating exogenous and endogenous modes of temporal attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 560–572. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029023
  • Los, S. A., & Schut, M. L. (2008). The effective time course of preparation. Cognitive Psychology, 57(1), 20–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.11.001
  • Loveless, N. E., & Sanford, A. J. (1975). The impact of warning signal intensity on reaction time and components of the contingent negative variation. Biological Psychology, 2(3), 217–226. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(75)90021-6
  • McCormick, C. R., Redden, R. S., Hurst, A. J., & Klein, R. M. (2019). On the selection of endogenous and exogenous signals. Royal Society Open Science, 6(11), 190134. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.190134
  • McCormick, C. R., Redden, R. S., Lawrence, M. A., & Klein, R. M. (2017). On the time-course of cued temporal attention. Poster Presented at the Meeting of the Canadian Society of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.16551.75682
  • McCormick, C. R., Redden, R. S., Lawrence, M. A., & Klein, R. M. (2018). The independence of endogenous and exogenous temporal attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(8), 1885–1891. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1575-y
  • Menceloglu, M., Suzuki, S., & Song, J. H. (2021). Revealing the effects of temporal orienting of attention on response conflict using continuous movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 83(4), 1463–1478. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02235-4
  • Nickerson, R. S., & Burnham, D. W. (1969). Response times with nonaging foreperiods. Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 79(3, Pt.1), 452–457. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026889
  • Niemi, P. (1982). Does increasing the warning signal intensity decrease or increase simple reaction time? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 23(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1982.tb00407.x
  • Nobre, A. C., & van Ede, F. (2018). Anticipated moments: Temporal structure in attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(1), 34–48. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.141
  • Nobre, A. C., & Van Ede, F. (2023). Attention in flux. Neuron, 111(7), 971–986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2023.02.032
  • Posner, M. I. (1975). Psychobiology of attention. Handbook of Psychobiology, 441–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-278656-3.50019-3
  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231
  • Posner, M. I., Klein, R., Summers, J., & Buggie, S. (1973). On the selection of signals. Memory & Cognition, 1(1), 2–12. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198062
  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
  • R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1967). Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 74(1), 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024109
  • Rohenkohl, G., Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (2011). Behavioural dissociation between exogenous and endogenous temporal orienting of attention. PLoS One, 6(1). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014620
  • Royall, R. (1997). Statistical evidence: A likelihood paradigm. Chapman and Hall. CRC Press.
  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30(0), 276–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9
  • Vangkilde, S., Coull, J. T., & Bundesen, C. (2012). Great expectations: Temporal expectation modulates perceptual processing speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1183–1191. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026343
  • Weinbach, N., & Henik, A. (2012). Temporal orienting and alerting – The same or different? Frontiers in Psychology, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00236
  • Yeshurun, Y., & Tkacz-Domb, S. (2021). The time-course of endogenous temporal attention-Super fast voluntary allocation of attention. Cognition, 206, 104506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104506

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.