1,735
Views
69
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Special Issue: Creativity and Insight Problem Solving

Reasoned connections: A dual-process perspective on creative thought

, , &

References

  • Aiello, D., Jarosz, A. F., Cushen, P. J., & Wiley, J. (2012). Firing the executive: When an analytic approach does and does not help problem solving. Journal of Problem Solving, 4, Article 6, 116–127.
  • Allen, A. P., & Thomas, K. E. (2011). A dual process account of creative thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 109–118.
  • Atchley, R., Strayer, D., & Atchley, P. (2012). Creativity in the wild: Improving creative reasoning through immersion in natural settings. PLoS ONE, 7, 1–3.
  • Ball, L., & Stevens, A. (2009). Evidence for a verbally-based analytic component to insight problem solving. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1060–1065). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 309–319.
  • Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 480–485.
  • Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 634–639.
  • Bunge, S. A., Wendelken, C., Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2005). Analogical reasoning and prefrontal cortex: Evidence for separable retrieval and integration mechanisms. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 239–249.
  • Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The rational imagination: How people create alternatives to reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Chein, J. M., & Weisberg, R. W. (2014). Working memory, insight, and restructuring in verbal problems: Analysis of compound remote associate problems. Memory & Cognition, 42, 67–83.
  • de Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 656–669.
  • De Neys, W., & Glumicic, T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition, 106, 1248–1299.
  • Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1011–1026.
  • Dorfman, J., Shames, V. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Intuition, incubation, and insight: Implicit cognition in problem solving. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Implicit cognition (pp. 257–296). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Dunbar, K., & Blanchette, I. (2001). The in vivo/in vitro approach to cognition: The case of analogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 334–339.
  • Evans, St. J. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 8, 223–241.
  • Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25–42.
  • Gabora, L. (2003). Contextual focus: A cognitive explanation for the cultural transition of the Middle/Upper Paleolithic. In R. Alterman & D. Hirsch (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 432–437). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Gabora, L. (2010). Revenge of the ‘neurds’: Characterizing creative thought in terms of the structure and dynamics of human memory. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 1–13.
  • Galton, F. (1865). Heredity talent and character. Macmillan's Magazine, 12, 157–166, 318–327.
  • Green, A., Cohen, M., Kim, J., & Gray, J. (2012). An explicit cue improves creative analogical reasoning. Intelligence, 40, 598–603.
  • Green, A. E., Fugelsang, J. A., Kraemer, D. J. M., Gray, J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2010). Connecting long distance: Semantic distance in analogical reasoning modulates frontopolar cortex activity. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 70–76.
  • Green, A. E., Fugelsang, J. A., Kraemer, D. J. M., Gray, J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2012). Neural correlates of creativity in analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 38, 264–272.
  • Green, A., Fugelsang, J., Kraemer, D., Shamosh, N., & Dunbar, K. (2006). The dynamic role of prefrontal cortex in reasoning and abstract thought. Brain Research, 1096, 125–137.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Hennessey B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 569–598.
  • Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1994). Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Huang, M. H., & Hauser, R. M. (1998). Trends in Black–White test score differentials: II the WORDSUM vocabulary test. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 303–332). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206–1217.
  • Jarosz, A. F., Colflesh, G. J. H., & Wiley, J. (2012). Uncorking the muse: Alcohol intoxication facilitates creative problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 487–493.
  • Kounios J., & Beaman M. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 71–93.
  • Malhorta, N., Krosnick, J. A., & Haertel, E. (2007). The psychometric properties of the GSS Wordsum vocabulary test (GSS Methodology Report No. 111). Chicago, IL: NORC.
  • Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232.
  • Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39, 36–45.
  • Ouspensky, P. D. (1950). The psychology of man's possible evolution. New York, NY: Hedgehog Press.
  • Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 972–987.
  • Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–178.
  • Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2013a). Cognitive style and religiosity: The role of conflict detection. Memory & Cognition, 42, 1–10.
  • Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J., Barr, N., Koehler, D., & Fugelsang, J. (2013b). The role of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking & Reasoning, 20, 188–214.
  • Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J., Seli, P., Koehler, D., & Fugelsang, J. (2012). Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition, 123, 335–346.
  • Schlegel, A., Kohler, P. J., Fogelson, S. V., Alexander, P., Konuthula, D., & Tse, P. U. (2013). Network structure and dynamics of the mental workspace. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 16277–16282.
  • Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., Black, W. C., & Welch, H. G. (1997). The role of numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography. Annals of Internal Medicine, 127, 966–972.
  • Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2012). Making creative metaphors: The importance of fluid intelligence for creative thought. Intelligence, 40, 343–351.
  • Slepian, M. L., & Ambady, N. (2012). Fluid movement and creativity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 625–629.
  • Slepian, M. L., Weisbuch, M., Rutchick, A. M., Newman, L. S., & Ambady, N. (2010). Shedding light on insight: Priming bright ideas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 696–700.
  • Spellman, B. A., Holyoak, K. J., & Morrison, R. G. (2001) Analogical priming via semantic relations. Memory and Cognition, 29, 383–393.
  • Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot's rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–726.
  • Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1275–1289.
  • Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. (2012). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 258–262.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.