Publication Cover
Neurocase
Behavior, Cognition and Neuroscience
Volume 22, 2016 - Issue 6: Music on the Mind
276
Views
10
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Article

Composing alarms: considering the musical aspects of auditory alarm design

&
Pages 566-576 | Received 28 Mar 2016, Accepted 04 Oct 2016, Published online: 21 Nov 2016

References

  • Ahlback, S. (2007). Melodic similarity as a determinant of melody structure. Musicae Scientiae, 11, 235–280. doi:10.1177/102986490701100110
  • Bartholomeus, B., & Doehring, D. (1971). Effect of stimulus order on verbal and nonverbal visual-auditory association. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 33, 891–897. doi:10.2466/pms.1971.33.3.891
  • Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. (1988). Scale structure and similarity of melodies. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 285–314. doi:10.2307/40285401
  • Cuddy, L. L., & Lyons, H. I. (1981). Musical pattern recognition: A comparison of listening to and studying tonal structures and tonal ambiguities. Psychomusicology: A Journal of Research in Music Cognition, 1, 15–33. doi:10.1037/h0094283
  • Donchin, Y., Gopher, D., Olin, M., Badihi, Y., Biesky, M., Sprung, C. L., … Cotev, S. (2003). A look into the nature and causes of human errors in the intensive care unit. Quality and Safety in Health Care, 12, 143–148. doi:10.1136/qhc.12.2.143
  • Dowling, W. J. (1971). Recognition of inversions of melodies and melodic contours. Perception & Psychophysics, 9, 348–349. doi:10.3758/BF03212663
  • Dowling, W. J. (1978). Scale and contour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychological Review, 85, 341–354. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.85.4.341
  • Dowling, W. J. (1991). Tonal strength and melody recognition after long and short delays. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 305–313. doi:10.3758/BF0321222
  • Dowling, W. J., & Bartlett, J. C. (1981). The importance of interval information in long-term memory for melodies. Psychomusicology: A Journal of Research in Music Cognition, 1, 30–49. doi:10.1037/h0094275
  • Dowling, W. J., & Fujitani, D. S. (1971). Contour, interval, and pitch recognition in memory for melodies. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 524–531. doi:10.1121/1.1912382
  • Edworthy, J. (1982). Pitch and contour in music processing. Psychomusicology: A Journal of Research in Music Cognition, 2, 44–46. doi:10.1037/h0094262
  • Edworthy, J. (1985). Interval and contour in melody processing. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 375–388. doi:10.2307/40285305
  • Edworthy, J. (1994). The design and implementation of non-verbal auditory warnings. Applied Ergonomics, 25, 202–210. doi:10.1016/0003-6870(94)90001-9
  • Edworthy, J. (2011). Designing effective alarm sounds. Biomedical Instrumentation & Technmelodic memory: A new, computationalology, 45, 290–294. doi:10.2345/0899-8205-45.4.290
  • Edworthy, J., & Hellier, E. (2005). Fewer but better auditory alarms will improve patient safety. Quality & Safety in Health Care, 14, 212–215. doi:10.1136/qshc.2004.013052
  • Edworthy, J., & Hellier, E. (2006). Alarms and human behaviour: Implications for medical alarms. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 97, 12–17. doi:10.1093/bja/ael114
  • Edworthy, J., Hellier, E., Titchener, K., Naweed, A., & Roels, R. (2011). Heterogeneity in auditory alarm sets makes them easier to learn. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 41, 136–146. doi:10.1016/j.ergon.2010.12.004
  • Edworthy, J., & Stanton, N. (1995). A user-centred approach to the design and evaluation of auditory warning signals: 1. Methodology. Ergonomics, 38, 2262–2280. doi:10.1080/00140139508925267
  • Godley, R., Estes, R., & Fournet, G. (1984). Paired-associate learning as a function of age and mode of presentation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59, 959–965. doi:10.2466/pms.1984.59.3.959
  • Huron, D. (2008). A comparison of average pitch height and interval size in major- and minor-key themes: Evidence consistent with affect-related pitch prosody. Empirical Musicology Review, 3, 59–63.
  • Keller, P., & Stevens, C. J. (2004). Meaning from environmental sounds: Types of signal-referent relations and their effect on recognizing auditory icons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 3–12. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.10.1.3
  • Klingberg, T., & Roland, P. E. (1998). Right prefrontal activation during encoding, but not during retrieval, in a non-verbal paired-associates task. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 73–79. doi:10.1093/cercor/8.1.73
  • Krumhansl, C. L., & Kessler, E. J. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psychological Review, 89, 334–368. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.89.4.334
  • Lacherez, P., Seah, E. L., & Sanderson, P. M. (2007). Overlapping melodic alarms are almost indiscriminable. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 49, 637–645. doi:10.1518/001872007X215719
  • Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. K. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition, 8, 1–71. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(80)90015-3
  • Marvin, E. W., & Laprade, P. A. (1987). Relating musical contours: Extensions of a theory for contour. Journal of Music Theory, 31, 225–267. doi:10.2307/843709
  • Massaro, D. W., Kallman, H. J., & Kelly, J. L. (1980). The role of tone height, melodic contour, and tone chroma in melody recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 6, 77–90. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.6.1.77
  • Mikumo, M. (1992). Encoding strategies for tonal and atonal melodies. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 10, 73–81. doi:10.2307/40285539
  • Morton-Evans, A., & Hensley, R. (1978). Paired associate learning in early infantile autism and receptive developmental aphasia. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 61–69. doi:10.1007/BF01550278
  • Müllensiefen, D., & Halpern, A. R. (2014). The role of features and context in recognition of novel melodies. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31, 418–435. doi:10.1525/mp.2014.31.5.418
  • Müllensiefen, D., & Wiggins, G. (2011). Sloboda and Parker ’s recall paradigm for melodic memory: A new, computational perspective. In I. Deliège & J. Davidso (Eds.), Essays in honour of John Sloboda (pp. 161–186). New York: Oxford University.
  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Patterson, R. (1990). Auditory warning sounds in the work environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 327, 485–492. doi:10.1098/rstb.1990.0091
  • Phansalkar, S., Edworthy, J., Hellier, E., Seger, D. L., Schedlbauer, A., Avery, A. J., & Bates, D. W. (2010). A review of human factors principles for the design and implementation of medication safety alerts in clinical information systems. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 17, 493–501. doi:10.1136/jamia.2010.005264
  • Roediger, H. L. (2008). Relativity of remembering: Why the laws of memory vanished. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 225–254. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190139
  • Sanderson, P. M., Liu, D., & Jenkins, S. A. (2009). Auditory displays in anesthesiology. Current Opinion in Anaesthesiology, 22, 788–795. doi:10.1097/ACO.0b013e3283326a2f
  • Sanderson, P. M., Wee, A. N., & Lacherez, P. (2006). Learnability and discriminability of melodic medical equipment alarms. Anaesthesia, 61, 142–147. doi:10.1111/ana.2006.61.issue-2
  • Schulkind, M. D., Posner, R. J., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Musical features that facilitate melody identification: How do you know it’s “your” song when they finally play it? Music Perception, 21, 217–249. doi:10.1525/mp.2003.21.issue-2
  • Schutz, M., Stefanucci, J. K., Baum, S., & Roth, A. (in press). Name that percussive tune: Associative memory and amplitude envelope. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1182562
  • Tan, S.-L., Spackman, M. P., & Bezdek, M. A. (2007). Viewers’ interpretations of film characters’ emotions: Effects of presenting film music before or after a character is shown. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 135–152. doi:10.1525/mp.2007.25.2.135
  • Taylor, J. G., Horwitz, B., Shah, N. J., Fellenz, W. M. H., & Krause, J. B. (2000). Decomposing memory: Functional assignments and brain traffic in paired word associate learning. Neural Networks, 13, 923–940. doi:10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00054-X
  • Vitouch, O. (2001). When your ear sets the stage: Musical context effects in film perception. Psychology of Music, 29, 70–83. doi:10.1177/0305735601291005
  • Wakefield, C. E., Homewood, J., & Taylor, A. J. (2004). Cognitive compensations for blindness in children: An investigation using odour naming. Perception, 33, 429–442. doi:10.1068/p5001
  • Wee, A. N., & Sanderson, P. M. (2008). Are melodic medical equipment alarms easily learned? Anesthesia & Analgesia, 106, 501–508. doi:10.1213/01.ane.0000286148.58823.6c

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.