Publication Cover
Laterality
Asymmetries of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognition
Volume 25, 2020 - Issue 4
255
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Measuring infant handedness reliably from reaching: A systematic review

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 430-454 | Received 29 Aug 2019, Accepted 30 Jan 2020, Published online: 16 Feb 2020

References

  • Annett, M. (1985). Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Annett, M. (2002). Handedness and brain asymmetry: The right shift theory. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
  • Atun-Einy, O., Berger, S. E., Ducz, J., & Sher, A. (2014). Strength of infants’ bimanual reaching patterns is related to the onset of upright locomotion. Infancy, 19(1), 82–102. doi: 10.1111/infa.12030
  • Babik, I., Campbell, J. M., & Michel, G. F. (2014). Postural influences on the development of infant lateralized and symmetric hand-use. Child Development, 85(1), 294–307. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12121
  • Baldwin, J. M. (1890). Origin of right or left handedness. Science, 404, 247–248. doi: 10.1126/science.ns-16.404.247
  • Bates, E., O’Connell, B., Vaid, J., Sledge, P., & Oakes, L. (1986). Language and hand preference in early development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2(1), 1–15. doi: 10.1080/87565648609540323
  • Benson, J. B., Cherny, S. S., Haith, M. M., & Fulker, D. W. (1993). Rapid assessment of infant predictors of adult Iq: Midtwin–midparent analyses. Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 434–447. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.3.434
  • Berger, S. E., Friedman, R., & Polis, M. C. (2011). The role of locomotor posture and experience on handedness and footedness in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(3), 472–480. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.05.003
  • Bresson, F., Maury, L., Pieraut-Le Bonniec, G., & de Schonen, S. (1977). Organization and lateralization of reaching in infants: An instance of asymmetric functions in hands collaboration. Neuropsychologia, 15(2), 311–320. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(77)90040-9
  • Butterworth, G., & Morissette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14(3), 219–231. doi: 10.1080/02646839608404519
  • Campbell, J. M., Marcinowski, E. C., Babik, I., & Michel, G. F. (2015). The influence of a hand preference for acquiring objects on the development of a hand preference for unimanual manipulation from 6 to 14 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 39, 107–117. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.013
  • Campbell, J. M., Marcinowski, E. C., Latta, J., & Michel, G. F. (2015). Different assessment tasks produce different estimates of handedness stability during the eight to 14 month age period. Infant Behavior & Development, 39, 67–80. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.003
  • Campbell, J. M., Marcinowski, E. C., & Michel, G. F. (2018). The development of neuromotor skills and hand preference during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 60(2), 165–175. doi: 10.1002/dev.21591
  • Caplan, P. J., & Kinsbourne, M. (1976). Baby drops the rattle: Asymmetry of duration of grasp by infants. Child Development, 532–534. doi: 10.2307/1128815
  • Carlson, D. F., & Harris, L. J. (1985). Development of the infant’s hand preference for visually directed reaching: Preliminary report of a longitudinal study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6, 158–174. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(198523)6:3<158::AID-IMHJ2280060307>3.0.CO;2-X
  • Chen, C.-Y., Tafone, S., Lo, W., & Heathcock, J. C. (2015). Perinatal stroke causes abnormal trajectory and laterality in reaching during early infancy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 38, 301–308. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.11.014
  • Cochet, H. (2012). Development of hand preference for object-directed actions and pointing gestures: A longitudinal study between 15 and 25 months of age. Developmental Psychobiology, 54(1), 105–111. doi: 10.1002/dev.20576
  • Cochet, H., & Vauclair, J. (2010). Pointing gestures produced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: Different functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. Infant Behavior & Development, 33(4), 431–441. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.009
  • Cohen, A. I. (1966). Hand preference and developmental status of infants. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 108(2), 337–345. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1966.10532792
  • Corbetta, D., & Bojczyk, K. E. (2002). Infants return to two-handed reaching when they are learning to walk. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34(1), 83–95. doi: 10.1080/00222890209601933
  • Corbetta, D., Friedman, D. R., & Bell, M. A. (2014). Brain reorganization as a function of walking experience in 12-month-old infants: Implications for the development of manual laterality. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 245. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00245
  • Corbetta, D., & Thelen, E. (1996). The developmental origins of bimanual coordination: A dynamic perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 505–522. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.502
  • Corbetta, D., & Thelen, E. (1999). Lateral biases and fluctuations in infants’ spontaneous arm movements and reaching. Developmental Psychobiology, 34(4), 237–255. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199905)34:2<237::AID-DEV1>3.0.CO;2-%23
  • Corbetta, D., Williams, J., & Snapp-Childs, W. (2006). Plasticity in the development of handedness: Evidence from normal development and early asymmetric brain injury. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(6), 460–471. doi: 10.1002/dev.20164
  • Cornwell, K. S., Harris, L. J., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1991). Task effects in the development of hand preference in 9-, 13-, and 20-month-old infant girls. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(1), 19–34. doi: 10.1080/87565649109540475
  • Coryell, J. F., & Michel, G. F. (1978). How supine postural preferences of infants can contribute toward the development of handedness. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 245–257. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(78)80036-8
  • Crichton-Browne, J. (1907). Dexterity and the bend sinister. Proceedings of the Royal Institution of Great Britain, 18, 623–652.
  • Dearborn, G. V. N. (1910). Moto-sensory development: Observations on the first three years of a child. Baltimore, MD: Warwick & York.
  • Domellöf, E., Barbu-Roth, M., Ronnqvist, L., Jacquet, A. Y., & Fagard, J. (2015). Infant manual performance during reaching and grasping for objects moving in depth. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1142. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01142
  • Ekberg, T. L., Rosander, K., Von Hofsten, C., Olsson, U., Soska, K. C., & Adolph, K. E. (2013). Dynamic reaching in infants during binocular and monocular viewing. Experimental Brain Research, 229(1), 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3588-z
  • Esseily, R., Jacquet, A.-Y., & Fagard, J. (2011). Handedness for grasping objects and pointing and the development of language in 14-month-old infants. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 16(5), 565–585. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2010.499911
  • Fagard, J., & Lemoine, C. (2006). The role of imitation in the stabilization of handedness during infancy. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 5(4), 519–533. doi: 10.1142/S0219635206001343
  • Fagard, J., & Lockman, J. J. (2005). The effect of task constraints on infants’(bi) manual strategy for grasping and exploring objects. Infant Behavior and Development, 28(3), 305–315. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.005
  • Fagard, J., Margules, S., Lopez, C., Granjon, L., & Huet, V. (2017). How should we test infant handedness? Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 22(3), 294–312. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2016.1192186
  • Fagard, J., & Marks, A. (2000). Unimanual and bimanual tasks and the assessment of handedness in toddlers. Developmental Science, 3(2), 137–147. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00107
  • Fagard, J., Sirri, L., & Rama, P. (2014). Effect of handedness on the occurrence of semantic N400 priming effect in 18- and 24-month-old children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 355. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00355
  • Fagard, J., Spelke, E., & von Hofsten, C. (2009). Reaching and grasping a moving object in 6-, 8-, and 10-month-old infants: Laterality and performance. Infant Behavior & Development, 32(2), 137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.002
  • Fenton, J. C. (1925). A practical psychology of babyhood. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Company.
  • Ferre, C. L., Babik, I., & Michel, G. F. (2010). Development of infant prehension handedness: A longitudinal analysis during the 6- to 14-month age period. Infant Behavior & Development, 33(4), 492–502. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.06.002
  • Flament, F. (1974). Practical intelligence and laterality: A genetic study of synergy and of hand preference in the neonate. Bulletin de Psychologie, 27, 681–684.
  • Geerts, W. K., Einspieler, C., Dibiasi, J., Garzarolli, B., & Bos, A. F. (2003). Development of manipulative hand movements during the second year of life. Early Human Development, 75(1–2), 91–103. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.09.006
  • Gesell, A., & Ames, L. B. (1947). The development of handedness. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 70(2), 155–175. doi: 10.1080/08856559.1947.10533403
  • Giesecke, M. (1936). The genesis of hand preference. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1(5), i-102. doi: 10.2307/1165415
  • Goldfield, E. C. (1989). Transition from rocking to crawling: Postural constraints on infant movement. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 913. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.913
  • Goldfield, E. C., & Michel, G. F. (1986). The ontogeny of infant bimanual reaching during the first year. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(1), 81–89. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(86)90040-8
  • Goodwin, R. S., & Michel, G. F. (1981). Head orientation position during birth and in infant neonatal period, and hand preference at nineteen weeks. Child Development, 819–826. doi: 10.2307/1129082
  • Harkins, D. A., & Michel, G. F. (1988). Evidence for a maternal effect on infant hand-use preferences. Developmental Psychobiology, 21(6), 535–541. doi: 10.1002/dev.420210604
  • Harris, L. J. (2003). What to do about your child’s handedness? Advice from five eighteenth-century authors, and some questions for today. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 8(2), 099–120. doi: 10.1080/713754480
  • Hinojosa, T., Sheu, C. F., & Michel, G. F. (2003). Infant hand-use preferences for grasping objects contributes to the development of a hand-use preference for manipulating objects. Developmental Psychobiology, 43(4), 328–334. doi: 10.1002/dev.10142
  • Hopkins, W. D. (2013). Comparing human and nonhuman primate handedness: Challenges and a modest proposal for consensus. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(6), 621–636. doi: 10.1002/dev.21139
  • Humphrey, D. E., & Humphrey, G. K. (1987). Sex differences in infant reaching. Neuropsychologia, 25(6), 971–975. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90101-1
  • Jacobsohn, L., Rodrigues, P., Vasconcelos, O., Corbetta, D., & Barreiros, J. (2014). Lateral manual asymmetries: A longitudinal study from birth to 24 months. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(1), 58–72. doi: 10.1002/dev.21091
  • Jacquet, A. Y., Esseily, R., Rider, D., & Fagard, J. (2012). Handedness for grasping objects and declarative pointing: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychobiology, 54(1), 36–46. doi: 10.1002/dev.20572
  • Kohen-Raz, R. (1966). The ring-cube test: A brief time sampling method for assessing primary development of coordinated bilateral grasp responses in infancy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23, 675. doi: 10.2466/pms.1966.23.3.675
  • Konishi, Y., Mikawa, H., & Suzuki, J. (1986). Asymmetrical head-turning of preterm infants: Some effects on later postural and functional lateralities. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 28(4), 450–457. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1986.tb14282.x
  • Kotwica, K. A., Ferre, C. L., & Michel, G. F. (2008). Relation of stable hand-use preferences to the development of skill for managing multiple objects from 7 to 13 months of age. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(5), 519–529. doi: 10.1002/dev.20311
  • Lewkowicz, D. J., & Turkewitz, G. (1982). Influence of hemispheric specialization in sensory processing on reaching in infants: Age and gender related effects. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 301–308. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.301
  • Lippman, H. S. (1927). Certain behavior responses in early infancy. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 34(3), 424–440. doi: 10.1080/08856559.1927.10532394
  • Lynch, A., Lee, H. M., Bhat, A., & Galloway, J. C. (2008). No stable arm preference during the pre-reaching period: A comparison of right and left hand kinematics with and without a toy present. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(4), 390–398. doi: 10.1002/dev.20297
  • Major, D. R. (1906). First steps in mental growth: A series of studies in the psychology of infancy. New York, NY: Macmillan.
  • Marcinowski, E. C., Campbell, J. M., Faldowski, R. A., & Michel, G. F. (2016). Do hand preferences predict stacking skill during infancy? Developmental Psychobiology, 58(8), 958–967. doi: 10.1002/dev.21426
  • Marschik, P. B., Einspieler, C., Strohmeier, A., Plienegger, J., Garzarolli, B., & Prechtl, H. F. (2008). From the reaching behavior at 5 months of age to hand preference at preschool age. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(5), 511–518. doi: 10.1002/dev.20307
  • Marsden, R. E. (1903). Discussion and apparatus: A study of the early color sense. Psychological Review, 10(1), 37. doi: 10.1037/h0071933
  • McCarty, M., Clifton, R., & Collard, R. (1999). Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 1091–1101. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1091
  • McCormick, C. M., & Maurer, D. M. (1988). Unimanual hand preferences in 6-month-olds: Consistency and relation to familial-handedness. Infant Behavior and Development, 11(1), 21–29. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(88)80013-4
  • McDonnell, P. M. (1979). Patterns of eye–hand coordination in the first year of life. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 33(4), 253. doi: 10.1037/h0081724
  • Meunier, H., Fagard, J., Maugard, A., Briseno, M., Fizet, J., Canteloup, C., … Vauclair, J. (2013). Patterns of hemispheric specialization for a communicative gesture in different primate species. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(6), 662–671. doi: 10.1002/dev.21151
  • Meunier, H., Vauclair, J., & Fagard, J. (2012). Human infants and baboons show the same pattern of handedness for a communicative gesture. PLoS One, 7(3), e33959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033959
  • Michel, G. F. (1981). Right-handedness: A consequence of infant supine head-orientation preference. Science, 212(4495), 685–687. doi: 10.1126/science.7221558
  • Michel, G. F. (1992). Maternal influences on infant hand-use during play with toys. Behavior Genetics, 22(2), 163–176. doi: 10.1007/BF01066995
  • Michel, G. F. (2002). Development of infant handedness. In D. J. Lewkowicz & R. Lickliter (Eds.), Conceptions of development: Lessons from the laboratory (pp. 165–186). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Michel, G. F. (2018). How might the relation of the development of hand preferences to the development of cognitive functions be examined during infancy: A sketch? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11, 739. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00739
  • Michel, G. F., Babik, I., Sheu, C. F., & Campbell, J. M. (2014). Latent classes in the developmental trajectories of infant handedness. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 349–359. doi: 10.1037/a0033312
  • Michel, G. F., & Harkins, D. A. (1986). Postural and lateral asymmetries in the ontogeny of handedness during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 19(3), 247–258. doi: 10.1002/dev.420190310
  • Michel, G. F., Ovrut, M. R., & Harkins, D. A. (1985). Hand-use preference for reaching and object manipulation in 6-month-old through 13-month-old infants. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs, 111(4), 407.
  • Michel, G. F., Sheu, C. F., & Brumley, M. R. (2002). Evidence of a right-shift factor affecting infant hand-use preferences from 7 to 11 months of age as revealed by latent class analysis. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 40(1), 1–13. doi: 10.1002/dev.10008
  • Michel, G. F., Tyler, A. N., Ferre, C., & Sheu, C. F. (2006). The manifestation of infant hand-use preferences when reaching for objects during the seven–to thirteen-month age period. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(6), 436–443. doi: 10.1002/dev.20161
  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151(4), 264–269. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135
  • Morange, F., & Bloch, H. (1996). Lateralization of the approach movement and the prehension movement in infants from 4 to 7 months. Early Development and Parenting: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 5(2), 81–92. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199606)5:2<81::AID-EDP119>3.0.CO;2-M
  • Morange-Majoux, F. (2011). Manual exploration of consistency (soft vs hard) and handedness in infants from 4 to 6 months old. Laterality, 16(3), 292–312. doi: 10.1080/13576500903553689
  • Morange-Majoux, F., & Dellatolas, G. (2010). Right-left approach and reaching arm movements of 4-month infants in free and constrained conditions. Brain and Cognition, 72(3), 419–422. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.12.002
  • Morange-Majoux, F., & Devouche, E. (2014). Social encouragement can influence manual preference in 6 month-old-infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1225. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01225
  • Morange-Majoux, F., Lemoine, C., & Dellatolas, G. (2013). Early manifestations of manual specialisation in infants: A longitudinal study from 20 to 30 weeks. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 18(2), 231–250. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2012.660163
  • Morange-Majoux, F., Peze, A., & Bloch, H. (2000). Organisation of left and right hand movement in a prehension task: A longitudinal study from 20 to 32 weeks. Laterality, 5, 351–362. doi: 10.1080/713754386
  • Mumford, K. H., & Kita, S. (2016). At 10-12 months, pointing gesture handedness predicts the size of receptive vocabularies. Infancy. doi: 10.1111/infa.12138
  • Nelson, E. L., Campbell, J. M., & Michel, G. F. (2013). Unimanual to bimanual: Tracking the development of handedness from 6 to 24 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 36(2), 181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.01.009
  • Nelson, E. L., Campbell, J. M., & Michel, G. F. (2014). Early handedness in infancy predicts language ability in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 809–814. doi: 10.1037/a0033803
  • Nelson, E. L., Konidaris, G. D., & Berthier, N. E. (2014). Hand preference status and reach kinematics in infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 37(4), 615–623. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.08.013
  • Nice, M. M. (1918). Ambidexterity and delayed speech development. The Pedagogical Seminary, 25(2), 141–162. doi: 10.1080/08919402.1918.10534734
  • Ojima, R. (1986). Manual asymmetry and its significance in the development of an infant. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 34(3), 274–279. doi: 10.5926/jjep1953.34.3_274
  • Palmer, R. D. (1964). Development of a differentiated handedness. Psychological Bulletin, 62(4), 257. doi: 10.1037/h0046588
  • Papadatou-Pastou, M., Martin, M., Munafo, M. R., & Jones, G. V. (2008). Sex differences in left-handedness: A meta-analysis of 144 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134(5), 677–699. doi: 10.1037/a0012814
  • Papadatou-Pastou, M., Ntolka, E., Schmitz, J., Martin, M., Munafò, M., Ocklenburg, S., & Paracchini, S. (2020). Human handedness: A meta-analysis. PsyArxiv. doi:10.31234/osf.io/5gjac [Preprint].
  • Petkovic, M., Chokron, S., & Fagard, J. (2016). Visuo-manual coordination in preterm infants without neurological impairments. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 51–52, 76–88. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.01.010
  • Piek, J. P., Gasson, N., Barrett, N., & Case, I. (2002). Limb and gender differences in the development of coordination in early infancy. Human Movement Science, 21(5–6), 621–639. doi: 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00172-0
  • Pogetti, L. S., de Souza, R. M., Tudella, E., & Teixeira, L. A. (2014). Moving what is seen: Arm visibility modulates infants’ manual preference. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(5), 331–341. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2014.932359
  • Potier, C., Meguerditchian, A., & Fagard, J. (2013). Handedness for bimanual coordinated actions in infants as a function of grip morphology. Laterality, 18(5), 576–593. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2012.732077
  • Provins, K., Dalziel, F., & Higginbottom, G. (1987). Asymmetrical hand usage in infancy: An ethological approach. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(2), 165–172. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(87)90031-2
  • Ramsay, D. S. (1980). Onset of unimanual handedness in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 3(4), 377–385. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(80)80045-2
  • Ramsay, D. S. (1984). Onset of duplicated syllable babbling and unimanual handedness in infancy: Evidence for developmental change in hemispheric specialization? Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 64. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.64
  • Ramsay, D. S. (1985). Fluctuations in unimanual hand preference in infants following the onset of duplicated syllable babbling. Developmental Psychology, 21(2), 318–324. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.2.318
  • Ramsay, D. S., & Weber, S. L. (1986). Infants’ hand preference in a task involving complementary roles for the two hands. Child Development, 57, 300–307. doi: 10.2307/1130585
  • Ramsay, D. S., & Willis, M. P. (1984). Organization and lateralization of reaching in infants: An extension of Bresson et al. Neuropsychologia, 22(5), 639–641. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(84)90031-9
  • Rat-Fischer, L., O’Regan, J. K., & Fagard, J. (2013). Handedness in infants’ tool use. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(8), 860–868. doi: 10.1002/dev.21078
  • Rönnqvist, L., & Domellöf, E. (2006). Quantitative assessment of right and left reaching movements in infants: A longitudinal study from 6 to 36 months. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(6), 444–459. doi: 10.1002/dev.20160
  • Sacco, S., Moutard, M. L., & Fagard, J. (2006). Agenesis of the corpus callosum and the establishment of handedness. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(6), 472–481. doi: 10.1002/dev.20162
  • Sacrey, L. A., Arnold, B., Whishaw, I. Q., & Gonzalez, C. L. (2013). Precocious hand use preference in reach-to-eat behavior versus manual construction in 1- to 5-year-old children. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(8), 902–911. doi: 10.1002/dev.21083
  • Sacrey, L. A., Karl, J. M., & Whishaw, I. Q. (2012). Development of rotational movements, hand shaping, and accuracy in advance and withdrawal for the reach-to-eat movement in human infants aged 6-12 months. Infant Behavior & Development, 35(3), 543–560. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.05.006
  • Schmitz, J., Metz, G. A., Güntürkün, O., & Ocklenburg, S. (2017). Beyond the genome—towards an epigenetic understanding of handedness ontogenesis. Progress in Neurobiology, 159, 69–89. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.10.005
  • Scola, C., & Vauclair, J. (2010). Is infant holding-side bias related to motor asymmetries in mother and child? Developmental Psychobiology, 52(5), 475–486. doi: 10.1002/dev.20450
  • Seth, G. (1973). Eye-hand co-ordination and ‘handedness’: A developmental study of visuo-motor behaviour in infancy. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 43(1), 35–49. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1973.tb00735.x
  • Shinn, M. W. (1914). Notes on the development of a Child. Berkeley: University of California.
  • Shiotani, Y., Matsuzawa, S., Ikeda, H., Sawada, A., Okada, M., Kutsuki, A., & Tomiwa, K. (2010). [Laterality of upper extremity movements in infancy: Observations at 4 and 9 months of age]. No to Hattatsu  [Brain and Development], 42(4), 287–290.
  • Shucard, J. L., & Shucard, D. W. (1990). Auditory evoked potentials and hand preference in 6-month-old infants: Possible gender-related differences in cerebral organization. Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 923–930. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.6.923
  • Smith, L. G. (1917). A brief survey of right-and left-handedness. The Pedagogical Seminary, 24(1), 19–35. doi: 10.1080/08919402.1917.10534715
  • Sounalet, G. (1975). Preferential use of one hand and fine prehension between 7 and 12 months. Enfance, 2(1), 33–149. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1976-27610-001
  • Souza, R. M., de Azevedo Neto, R. M., Tudella, E., & Teixeira, L. A. (2012). Is early manual preference in infants defined by intermanual performance asymmetry in reaching? Infant Behavior & Development, 35(4), 742–750. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.06.007
  • Streri, A. (2002). Hand preference in 4-month-old infants: Global or local processing of objects in the haptic mode. Current Psychology Letters. Behaviour, Brain & Cognition, 7, 39–50.
  • Stroganova, T., Posikera, I., Pushina, N., & Orekhova, E. (2003). Lateralization of motor functions in early human ontogeny. Human Physiology, 29(1), 40–48. doi: 10.1023/A:1022096301176
  • Stroganova, T., Pushina, N., Orekhova, E., Posikera, I., & Tsetlin, M. (2004). Functional brain asymmetry and individual differences in hand preference in early ontogeny. Human Physiology, 30(1), 14–23. doi: 10.1023/B:HUMP.0000013759.88727.01
  • Sully, J. (1896). Extracts from a father’s diary. Studies of Childhood, 2, 399–488.
  • Suzuki, K., Ando, J., & Satou, N. (2009). Genetic effects on infant handedness under spatial constraint conditions. Developmental Psychobiology, 51(8), 605–615. doi: 10.1002/dev.20395
  • Tachibana, H. (2009). Functional asymmetry in an infant’s manual activity: A longitudinal stwudy from birth to one year. Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 55–65. doi:10.11201/jjdp.20.55
  • Tirosh, E., Stein, M., Harel, J., & Scher, A. (1999). Hand preference as related to development and behavior in infancy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89(2), 371–380. doi: 10.2466/pms.1999.89.2.371
  • Tirosh, E., Stein, M. M., & Harel, J. (1997). Relationship between left-handedness and allergic symptomatology. Journal of Child Neurology, 12(3), 165–168. doi: 10.1177/088307389701200302
  • Van Hof, P., Van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G. (2002). The relation of unimanual and bimanual reaching to crossing the midline. Child Development, 73(5), 1353–1362. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00476
  • Vauclair, J., & Cochet, H. (2013). Hand preference for pointing and language development in toddlers. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(7), 757–765. doi: 10.1002/dev.21073
  • Vauclair, J., & Imbault, J. (2009). Relationship between manual preferences for object manipulation and pointing gestures in infants and toddlers. Developmental Science, 12(6), 1060–1069. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00850.x
  • Wallace, B. C., Small, K., Brodley, C. E., Lau, J., & Trikalinos, T. A. (2012). Deploying an interactive machine learning system in an evidence-based practice center: Abstrackr. Proceedings of the 2nd ACM SIGHIT International Health Informatics Symposium, Miami, FL (pp. 819–824). New York, NY: ACM. doi:10.1145/2110363.2110464
  • Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism. New York, NY: W.W. Norton and Company.
  • Woolley, H. T. (1910). The development of right-handedness in a normal infant. Psychological Review, 17(1), 37–41. doi: 10.1037/h0074110
  • Xintian, L., Minggao, L., Huikun, G., & Kuihe, J. (1984). Experimental study on grasping with left or right hand by 6 mo. to 3-year-old children. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 16(2), 214–222. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1985-14122-001
  • Xintian, L., Minggao, L., Huikun, G., & Kuihe, J. (1991). Experimental study of grasping with the left or the right hand by children six months to three years old. International Journal of Mental Health, 20(1), 12–26. doi: 10.1080/00207411.1991.11449182
  • Young, A. W., Lock, A. J., & Service, V. (1985). Infants’ hand preferences for actions and gestures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 1(1), 17–27. doi: 10.1080/87565648509540295

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.