151
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The partially overlapping nature of inhibitory control and preparatory control during language and task switching

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon
Pages 410-426 | Received 13 Dec 2021, Accepted 27 Apr 2023, Published online: 18 May 2023

References

  • Blanco-Elorrieta, E., and A. Caramazza. 2021. “A Common Selection Mechanism at Each Linguistic Level in Bilingual and Monolingual Language Production.” Cognition 213: 104625. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104625.
  • Branzi, F., M. Calabria, M. Boscarino, and A. Costa. 2016. “On the Overlap Between Bilingual Language Control and Domain-General Executive Control.” Acta Psychologica 166: 21–30. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.03.001.
  • Calabria, M., F. Branzi, P. Marne, M. Hernández, and A. Costa. 2015. “Age-Related Effects Over Bilingual Language Control and Executive Control.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18 (1): 65–78. doi:10.1017/S1366728913000138.
  • Calabria, M., M. Hernández, F. Branzi, and A. Costa. 2012. “Qualitative Differences Between Bilingual Language Control and Executive Control: Evidence from Task-Switching.” Frontiers in Psychology 2: 399. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00399.
  • Chabal, S., and V. Marian. 2015. “Speakers of Different Languages Process the Visual World Differently.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 144: 539–550. doi:10.1037/xge0000075.
  • Chevalier, N., and A. Blaye. 2009. “Setting Goals to Switch Between Tasks: Effect of cue Transparency on Children’s Cognitive Flexibility.” Developmental Psychology 45 (3): 782–797. doi:10.1037/a0015409.
  • Cipolotti, L., E. K. Warrington, and B. Butterworth. 1995. “Selective Impairment in Manipulating Arabic Numerals.” Cortex 31 (1): 73–86. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80106-2.
  • Costa, A., M. Miozzo, and A. Caramazza. 1999. “Lexical Selection in Bilinguals: Do Words in the Bilingual’s two Lexicons Compete for Selection?” Journal of Memory and Language 41: 365–397. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2651.
  • Costa, A., and M. Santesteban. 2004. “Lexical Access in Bilingual Speech Production: Evidence from Language Switching in Highly Proficient Bilinguals and L2 Learners.” Journal of Memory and Language 50 (4): 491–511. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.002.
  • Costa, A., M. Santesteban, and I. Ivanova. 2006. “How Do Highly Proficient Bilinguals Control Their Lexicalization Process? Inhibitory and Language-Specific Selection Mechanisms are Both Functional.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 32 (5): 1057–1074. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1057.
  • Cui, J., Y. Zhang, D. Cheng, D. Li, and X. Zhou. 2017. “Visual Form Perception Can be Cognitive Correlate of Lower-Level Math Categories for Teenagers.” Frontiers in Psychology 8 (1336): 1–17. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01336.
  • De Baene, W., W. Duyck, M. Brass, and M. Carreiras. 2015. “Brain Circuit for Cognitive Control is Shared by Task and Language Switching.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27 (9): 1752–1765. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00817.
  • Declerck, M., J. Grainger, I. Koch, and A. Philipp. 2017. “Is Language Control Just a Form of Executive Control? Evidence for Overlapping Processes in Language Switching and Task Switching.” Journal of Memory and Language 95: 138–145. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2017.03.005.
  • Declerck, M., I. Ivanova, J. Grainger, and J. Duñabeitia. 2020. “Are Similar Control Processes Implemented During Single and Dual Language Production?” Evidence from Switching Between Speech Registers and Languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23 (3): 694–701. doi:10.1017/S1366728919000695.
  • Declerck, M., I. Koch, and A. Philipp. 2012. “Digits vs. Pictures: The Influence of Stimulus Type on Language Switching.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15 (4): 896–904. doi:10.1017/S1366728912000193.
  • Declerck, M., G. Meade, K. Midgley, P. Holcomb, A. Roelofs, and K. Emmorey. 2021. “On the Connection Between Language Control and Executive Control – An ERP Study.” Neurobiology of Language, 1–19. doi:10.1162/nol_a_00032.
  • Declerck, M., and A. Philipp. 2015. “A Review of Control Processes and Their Locus in Language Switching.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22: 1630–1645. doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0836-1.
  • Gollan, T., and V. Ferreira. 2009. “Should I Stay or Should I Switch? A Cost-Benefit Analysis of Voluntary Language Switching in Young and Aging Bilinguals.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35 (3): 640–665. doi:10.1037/a0014981.
  • Gollan, T., D. Kleinman, and C. Wierenga. 2014. “What's Easier: Doing What You Want, or Being Told What to do? Cued Versus Voluntary Language and Task Switching.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143 (6): 2167–2195. doi:10.1037/a0038006.
  • Graham, B., and A. Lavric. 2021. “Preparing to Switch Languages Versus Preparing to Switch Tasks: Which is More Effective?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, doi:10.1037/xge0001027.
  • Green, D. 1998. “Mental Control of the Bilingual Lexico-Semantic System.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1 (2): 67–81. doi:10.1017/S1366728998000133.
  • Herrera, A., and P. Macizo. 2010. “The Production of Symbolic and Non-Symbolic Numerals.” Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences 5: 926–934. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.212.
  • Kiesel, A., M. Steinhauser, M. Wendt, M. Falkenstein, K. Jost, A. Philipp, and I. Koch. 2010. “Control and Interference in Task Switching – A Review.” Psychological Bulletin 136: 849–874. doi:10.1037/a0019842.
  • Klecha, A. 2013. “Language and Task Switching in Polish-English Bilinguals.” Psychology of Language and Communication 17 (1): 17–36. doi:10.2478/plc-2013-0002.
  • Koch, I., M. Gade, S. Schuch, and A. Philipp. 2010. “The Role of Inhibition in Task Switching: A Review.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 17 (1): 1–14. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.1.1.
  • La Heij, W. 2005. “Selection Processes in Monolingual and Bilingual Lexical Access.” In The Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches, edited by J. Kroll, and A. de Groot, 289–307. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Lavric, A., A. Clapp, A. East, H. Elchlepp, and S. Monsell. 2019. “Is Preparing for a Language Switch Like Preparing for a Task Switch?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 45 (7): 1224–1233. doi:10.1037/xlm0000636.
  • Li, S., M. Botezatu, M. Zhang, and T. Guo. 2021. “Different Inhibitory Control Components Predict Different Levels of Language Control in Bilinguals.” Memory & Cognition, 1–13. doi:10.3758/s13421-020-01131-4.
  • Li, P., S. Sepanski, and X. Zhao. 2006. “Language History Questionnaire: A Web-Based Interface for Bilingual Research.” Behavior Research Methods 38 (2): 202–210. doi:10.3758/BF03192770.
  • Linck, J. A., J. W. Schwieter, and G. Sunderman. 2020. “The Differential Role of Executive Functions in the Cognitive Control of Language Switching.” Languages 5 (2): 1–20. doi:10.3390/languages5020019.
  • Liu, H., N. Fan, S. Rossi, P. Yao, and B. Chen. 2016. “The Effect of Cognitive Flexibility on Task Switching and Language Switching.” International Journal of Bilingualism 20 (5): 563–579. doi:10.1177/1367006915572400.
  • Liu, C., L. U. Jiao, Z. Wang, M. Wang, R. Wang, and Y. J. Wu. 2019. “Symmetries of Bilingual Language Switch Costs in Conflicting Versus Non-Conflicting Contexts.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22 (3): 624–636. doi:10.1017/S1366728918000494.
  • Ma, F., S. Li, and T. Guo. 2016. “Reactive and Proactive Control in Bilingual Word Production: An Investigation of Influential Factors.” Journal of Memory and Language 86: 35–59. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2015.08.004.
  • Macizo, P., and A. Herrera. 2011. “Cognitive Control in Number Processing: Evidence from the Unit–Decade Compatibility Effect.” Acta Psychologica 136 (1): 112–118. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.008.
  • Mayr, U., and R. Kliegl. 2003. “Differential Effects of Cue Changes and Task Changes on Task-Set Selection Costs.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 29 (3): 362. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.362.
  • Meuter, R., and A. Allport. 1999. “Bilingual Language Switching in Naming: Asymmetrical Costs of Language Selection.” Journal of Memory and Language 40 (1): 25–40. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2602.
  • Miyake, A., M. Emerson, F. Padilla, and J. Ahn. 2004. “Inner Speech as a Retrieval aid for Task Goals: The Effects of Cue Type and Articulatory Suppression in the Random Task Cuing Paradigm.” Acta Psychologica 115 (2-3): 123–142. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.004.
  • Monsell, S. 2003. “Task Switching.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (3): 134–140. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7.
  • Mosca, M., and H. Clahsen. 2016. “Examining Language Switching in Bilinguals: The Role of Preparation Time.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19 (2): 415–424. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000693.
  • Philipp, A., M. Gade, and I. Koch. 2007. “Inhibitory Processes in Language Switching: Evidence from Switching Language-Defined Response Sets.” European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19 (3): 395–416. doi:10.1080/09541440600758812.
  • Poulisse, N., and T. Bongaerts. 1994. “First Language Use in Second Language Production.” Applied Linguistics 15 (1): 36–57. doi:10.1093/applin/15.1.36.
  • Prior, A., and T. Gollan. 2011. “Good Language-Switchers are Good Task-Switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English Bilinguals.” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17 (4): 682–691. doi:10.1017/S1355617711000580.
  • Prior, A., and T. Gollan. 2013. “The Elusive Link Between Language Control and Executive Control: A Case of Limited Transfer.” Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25 (5): 622–645. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.821993.
  • Prior, A., and B. MacWhinney. 2010. “A Bilingual Advantage in Task Switching.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13 (2): 253–262. doi:10.1017/S1366728909990526.
  • Roelofs, A. 1998. “Lemma Selection Without Inhibition of Languages in Bilingual Speakers.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1 (2): 94–95. doi:10.1017/S1366728998000194.
  • Roelofs, A., V. Piai, G. Garrido Rodriguez, and D. Chwilla. 2016. “Electrophysiology of Cross-Language Interference and Facilitation in Picture Naming.” Cortex 76: 1–16. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.003.
  • Schwieter, J. W., and G. Sunderman. 2008. “Language Switching in Bilingual Speech Production: In Search of the Language-Specific Selection Mechanism.” The Mental Lexicon 3 (2): 214–238. doi:10.1075/ml.3.2.06sch.
  • Segal, D., A. Stasenko, and T. H. Gollan. 2019. “More Evidence That a Switch is not (Always) a Switch: Binning Bilinguals Reveals Dissociations Between Task and Language Switching.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 148 (3): 501–519. doi:10.1037/xge0000515.
  • Soveri, A., A. Rodriguez-Fornells, and M. Laine. 2011. “Is There a Relationship Between Language Switching and Executive Functions in Bilingualism? Introducing a Within Group Analysis Approach.” Frontiers in Psychology 2: 183. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183.
  • Stasenko, A., G. Matt, and T. Gollan. 2017. “A Relative Bilingual Advantage in Switching with Preparation: Nuanced Explorations of the Proposed Association Between Bilingualism and Task Switching.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 146 (11): 1527–1550. doi:10.1037/xge0000340.
  • Thomas, M., and A. Allport. 2000. “Language Switching Costs in Bilingual Visual Word Recognition.” Journal of Memory and Language 43 (1): 44–66. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2700.
  • Timmer, K., M. Calabria, F. M. Branzi, C. Baus, and A. Costa. 2018. “On the Reliability of Switching Costs Across Time and Domains.” Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1032. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01032.
  • Timmermeister, M., P. Leseman, F. Wijnen, and E. Blom. 2020. “No Bilingual Benefits Despite Relations Between Language Switching and Task Switching.” Frontiers in Psychology 11: 1832. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01832.
  • Verhoef, K., A. Roelofs, and D. J. Chwilla. 2009. “Role of Inhibition in Language Switching: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials in Overt Picture Naming.” Cognition 110 (1): 84–99. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.013.
  • Weissberger, G., T. Gollan, M. Bondi, L. Clark, and C. Wierenga. 2015. “Language and Task Switching in the Bilingual Brain: Bilinguals are Staying, not Switching, Experts.” Neuropsychologia 66: 193–203. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037.
  • Wiseheart, M., M. Viswanathan, and E. Bialystok. 2016. “Flexibility in Task Switching by Monolinguals and Bilinguals.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19 (1): 141–146. doi:10.1017/S1366728914000273.
  • Yang, H., A. Hartanto, and S. Yang. 2016. “The Complex Nature of Bilinguals’ Language Usage Modulates Task-Switching Outcomes.” Frontiers in Psychology 7: 560. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00560.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.