510
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

How language and inhibition influence analogical reasoning in children with or without developmental language disorder?

, ORCID Icon &
Pages 76-89 | Received 28 Dec 2018, Accepted 17 Sep 2019, Published online: 10 Oct 2019

References

  • Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559.
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Larsen, J. D. (2007). The phonological loop unmasked? A comment on the evidence for a “perceptual-gestural” alternative. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(4),497–504.
  • Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 575–589.
  • Balthazar, C. H. (2003). The word length effect in children with language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(6), 487–505.
  • Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., & Catalise‐2 Consortium. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068–1080.
  • Botting, N. (2005). Non‐verbal cognitive development and language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(3), 317–326.
  • Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Campoy, G., & Baddeley, A. (2008). Phonological and semantic strategies in immediate serial recall. Memory, 16(4), 329–340.
  • Chevrie-Muller, C., Maillart, C., Simon, A.-M., & Fournier, S. (2010). Batterie langage oral, langage écrit, mémoire, attention (2de éd. ed.). Montreuil, France: ECPA.
  • Christie, S., & Gentner, D. (2014). Language helps children succeed on a classic analogy task. Cognitive Science, 38(2), 383–397.
  • Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non‐word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(1), 1–40.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Cuperus, J., Vugs, B., Scheper, A., & Hendriks, M. (2014). Executive function behaviours in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 60(3), 132–143.
  • Dunn, L. M., Theriault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire en images peabody. Toronto, Canada: Psychan.
  • Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 148–168.
  • Fatzer, S. T., & Roebers, C. M. (2012). Language and executive functions: The effect of articulatory suppression on executive functioning in children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(4), 454–472.
  • Gentner, D., & Christie, S. (2010). Mutual bootstrapping between language and analogical processing. Language and Cognition, 2(2), 261–283.
  • Gentner, D., Simms, N., & Flusberg, S. (2009). Relational language helps children reason analogically. In Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1054–1059). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Gentner, D., & Smith, L. (2012). Analogical reasoning. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (2nd ed., pp. 130–136). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
  • Geurten, M., Catale, C., & Meulemans, T. (2016). Involvement of executive functions in children’s metamemory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(1), 70–80.
  • Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(2), 149–171.
  • Hofstadter, D., & Sander, E. (2013). L’Analogie: Cœur de la pensée. Paris, France: Odile Jacob Sciences.
  • Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822–1841.
  • Kemény, F., & Lukács, Á. (2010). Impaired procedural learning in language impairment: Results from probabilistic categorization. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 249–258.
  • Khomsi, A. (2001). ELO: Evaluation du langage oral. Paris, France: ECPS.
  • Kotovsky, L., & Gentner, D. (1996). Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Development, 67(6), 2797–2822.
  • Krzemien, M., Jemel, B., & Maillart, C. (2017). Analogical reasoning in children with specific language impairment: Evidence from a scene analogy task. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31, 573–588.
  • Kuusisto, M. A., Nieminen, P. E., Helminen, M. T., & Kleemola, L. (2017). Executive and intellectual functioning in school‐aged children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(2),127–136.
  • Larsen, J. D.,&Baddeley, A. (2003). Disruption of verbal STM by irrelevant speech, articulatory suppression, and manual tapping: Do they have a common source? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A,56(8), 1249–1268.
  • Lecocq, P. (1996). L’ECOSSE: Une épreuve de compréhension syntaxico-sémantique. Villeneuve-d’Ascq, France: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.
  • Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Leroy, S., Maillart, C., & Parisse, C. (2014). Analogical mapping across modalities in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(9), 2158–2171.
  • Leroy, S., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2012). Analogical reasoning in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(4), 380–395.
  • Leroy, S., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2014). Le manque de généralisation chez les enfants dysphasiques : Une étude longitudinale. ANAE, 131, 1–9.
  • Lidstone, J. S., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2010). The roles of private speech and inner speech in planning during middle childhood: Evidence from a dual task paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 438–451.
  • Lidstone, J. S., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2012). Verbal mediation of cognition in children with specific language impairment. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 651–660.
  • Logie, R. H., Della Sala, S., Wynn, V., & Baddeley, A. D. (2000). Visual similarity effects in immediate verbal serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 53(3), 626–646.
  • Lum, J. A., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2013). Long-term memory: A review and meta-analysis of studies of declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 33(4), 282–297.
  • Maguire, M. J., Schneider, J. M., Middleton, A. E., Ralph, Y., Lopez, M., Ackerman, R. A., & Abel, A. D. (2018). Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 679–695.
  • Masterson, J. J., Evans, L. H., & Aloia, M. (1993). Verbal analogical reasoning in children with language-learning disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 76–82.
  • Mussweiler, T., & Epstude, K. (2009). Relatively fast! Efficiency advantages of comparative thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(1), 1–21.
  • Niemi, J., Gundersen, H., Leppäsaari, T., & Hugdahl, K. (2003). Speech lateralization and attention/executive functions in a Finnish family with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(4), 457–464.
  • Nippold, M. A., Erskine, B. J., & Freed, D. B. (1988). Proportional and functional analogical reasoning in normal and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 53, 440–448.
  • Norris, D., Butterfield, S., Hall, J., & Page, M. P. (2018). Phonological recoding under articulatory suppression. Memory & Cognition, 46(2), 173–180.
  • Otsuka, Y., & Osaka, N. (2015). High-performers use the phonological loop less to process mental arithmetic during working memory tasks.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(5),878–886.
  • Poloczek, S.,Henry, L. A.,Messer, D. J., & Büttner, G. (2019).Do children use different forms of verbal rehearsal in serial picture recall tasks? A multi-method study. Memory, 27,758–771.
  • Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (2012). E-Prime 2.0. Retrieved from http://www.pstnet.com
  • R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  • Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S., & van der Lely, H. K. (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain, 136(2), 630–645.
  • Rao, K. V., & Baddeley, A. (2013). Raven’s matrices and working memory: A dual-task approach. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(10), 1881–1887.
  • Richland, L. E.,Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children’s development of analogical reasoning: Insights from scene analogy problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(3), 249–273.
  • Stigler, J. W., Lee, S. Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1986). Digit memory in Chinese and English: Evidence for a temporally limited store. Cognition, 23(1), 1–20.
  • Sullivan, J., & Barner, D. (2013). How are number words mapped to approximate magnitudes? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 389–402.
  • Thibaut, J.-P., French, R., & Vezneva, M. (2010). The development of analogy making in children: Cognitive load and executive functions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 1–19.
  • Thibaut, J. P., French, R. M., Vezneva, M., Gérard, Y., & Glady, Y. (2011). Semantic analogies by young children: Testing the role of inhibition. In B. Kokinov, A. Karmiloff-Smith, & N. J. Nersessian (Eds.), European perspectives on cognitive science (pp. 136–140). Sofia, Bulgaria: New Bulgarian University Press.
  • Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Xuyang, Z., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.
  • Vezneva, M. (2011). Développement du raisonnement analogique: Rôle de la composante exécutive d’inhibition (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France.
  • Vugs, B., Hendriks, M. P. H., Cuperus, J., & Knoors, H. (2017). Developmental associations between working memory and language in children with specific language impairment: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 2384–2394.
  • Waltz, J. A., Lau, A., Grewal, S. K., & Holyoak, K. J. (2000). The role of working memory in analogical mapping. Memory & Cognition, 28(7), 1205–1212.
  • Wechsler, D., & Naglieri, J. (2009). Echelle non verbale d’intelligence de Wechsler. Montreuil, France: ECPA.
  • Yang, H.-C., & Gray, S. (2017). Executive function in preschoolers with primary language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 379–392.
  • Zelaznik, H. N., & Goffman, L. (2010). Generalized motor abilities and timing behavior in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 383–393.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.