Publication Cover
Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition
A Journal on Normal and Dysfunctional Development
Volume 25, 2018 - Issue 3
2,376
Views
40
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Bilingualism and age are continuous variables that influence executive function

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 443-463 | Received 24 Sep 2016, Accepted 08 Apr 2017, Published online: 24 Apr 2017

References

  • Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Parris, B. A., & Weekes, B. S. (2015). Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia, 69, 201–210. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.040
  • Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., … Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4
  • Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959–963. doi:10.1002/ana.v75.6
  • Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V. (2012). Language learning and control in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognitive Science, 36, 1129–1147. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01243.x
  • Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 857–894. doi:10.1017/S0142716414000174
  • Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46, 93–105. doi:10.1037/a0015466
  • Bialystok, E. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a model for bilingualism: Commentary on Baum and Titone. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 899–902. doi:10.1017/S0142716414000198
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Binns, M. A., Ossher, L., & Freedman, M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: Evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology, 28, 290–304. doi:10.1037/neu0000023
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
  • Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29, 696–705. doi:10.1037/a0037254
  • Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402, 179–181. doi:10.1038/46035
  • Chertkow, H., Whitehead, V., Phillips, N., Wolfson, C., Atherton, J., & Bergman, H. (2010). Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: Evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 24, 118–125. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181ca1221
  • de Bruin, A., Roelofs, A., Dijkstra, T., & Fitzpatrick, I. (2014). Domain-general inhibition areas of the brain are involved in language switching: FMRI evidence from trilingual speakers. NeuroImage, 90, 348–359. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.049
  • Dijkstra, T. (2005). Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 179–201). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. doi:10.3758/BF03203267
  • Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2010). MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 226–241. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.1.226
  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101–135. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101
  • Gollan, T. H., Salmon, D. P., Montoya, R. I., & Galasko, D. R. (2011). Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3826–3830. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.041
  • Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G. H., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., & Cera, C. M. (2012). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594–615. doi:10.1017/S1366728911000332
  • Grainger, J., & Beauvillain, C. (1987). Language blocking and lexical access in bilinguals. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 39, 295–319. doi:10.1080/14640748708401788
  • Grainger, J. (1993). Visual word recognition in bilinguals. In R. Schreuder & B. Weltens (Eds.), The bilingual lexicon (pp. 11–26). John Benjamin Publishing. doi: 10.1075/sibil.6.
  • Grosjean, F. (2010). Bilingualism: Life and reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Guzmán-Vélez, E., & Tranel, D. (2015). Does bilingualism contribute to cognitive reserve? Cognitive and neural perspectives. Neuropsychology, 29, 139–150. doi:10.1037/neu0000105
  • Hehman, E., Stolier, R. M., & Freeman, J. B. (2015). Advanced mouse-tracking analytic techniques for enhancing psychological science. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18, 384–401. doi:10.1177/1368430214538325
  • Heidlmayr, K., Moutier, S., Hemforth, B., Tanzmeister, R., & Isel, F. (2014). Successive bilingualism and executive functions: The effect of second language use on inhibitory control in a behavioural Stroop Colour Word task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 630–645. doi:10.1017/S1366728913000539
  • Incera, S., Markis, T. A., & McLennan, C. T. (2013). Mouse-tracking reveals when the Stroop effect happens. Ohio Psychologist, 60, 33–34.
  • Incera, S., & McLennan, C. T. (2016a). Mouse tracking reveals that bilinguals behave like experts. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 610–620. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000218
  • Incera, S., & McLennan, C. T. (2016b). The time course of within- and between-language interference in bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 1–12.
  • Incera, S, Shah, A. P, McLennan, C. T, & Wetzel, M. T. (2017). Sentence context influences the subjective perception of foreign accents. Acta Psychologica, 172, 71–76. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.11.011
  • Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23, 70–78. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.70
  • Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2017). A behavioural and electrophysiological investigation of the effect of bilingualism on aging and cognitive control. Neuropsychologia, 94, 23–35. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.013
  • Krestar, M., Incera, S., & McLennan, C. T. (2013). Using mouse tracking to examine the time course of an auditory lexical decision task. Ohio Psychologist, 60, 29–32.
  • Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A. (2009). Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 381.
  • Ljungberg, J. K., Hansson, P., Andrés, P., Josefsson, M., & Nilsson, L. G. (2013). A longitudinal study of memory advantages in bilinguals. Plos One, 8, e73029. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0073029
  • Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605–621. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.795574
  • MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 19–40. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.19
  • Macnamara, B. N., & Conway, A. R. (2014). Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 520–525. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0524-y
  • Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 940–967. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)
  • Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between- language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97–115. doi:10.1017/S1366728903001068
  • Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1993). Bivariate median splits and spurious statistical significance. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 181–190. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.181
  • Mekala, S., Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shukla, A. K., … Kaul, S. (2013). Age of onset of dementia is delayed by multilingualism and advanced by stroke and rural dwelling independently. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 333, e344–e344. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2013.07.1268
  • Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton, Florida: CRC Press.
  • Nucci, M., Mapelli, D., & Mondini, S. (2012). The cognitive reserve questionnaire (CRIq): A new instrument for measuring the cognitive reserve. Aging Clinical Experimental Research, 24, 218–226.
  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
  • Padilla, C., Mendez, M. F., Jimenez, E. E., & Teng, E. (2016). Bilingualism in older Mexican-American immigrants is associated with higher scores on cognitive screening. BMC Geriatrics, 16, 1–9. doi:10.1186/s12877-016-0368-1
  • Park, D. C. (2000). Cognitive aging: A primer. New York: Psychology Press.
  • Pelham, S. D, & Abrams, L. (2014). Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 313–325. doi:10.1037/a0035224
  • Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2015). Bilingualism, dementia, cognitive and neural reserve. Current Opinion in Neurology, 28, 618–625. doi:10.1097/WCO.0000000000000267
  • Pontifex, M. B., Saliba, B. J., Raine, L. B., Picchietti, D. L., & Hillman, C. H. (2012). Acute exercise enhances inhibition in children with ADHD. Medicine and Science Sports in Exercise, 44, 221–222.
  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: A new look at old problems. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65, 405–415. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq035
  • Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood: The Seattle longitudinal study. Cambridge University Press.
  • Schwartz, A. I., & Kroll, J. F. (2006). Bilingual lexical activation in sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 197–212. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.004
  • Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Bilingualism and Cognitive Control, 183, 1–8.
  • Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). From The Cover: Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 10393–10398. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503903102
  • Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448–460. doi:10.1017/S1355617702813248
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651
  • Suarez, P. A., Gollan, T. H., Heaton, R., Grant, I., & Cherner, M. (2014). Second-language fluency predicts native language Stroop effects: Evidence from Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 342–348. doi:10.1017/S1355617714000058
  • Sylvain-Roy, S., & Belleville, S. (2015). Interindividual differences in attentional control profiles among younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 22, 259–279. doi:10.1080/13825585.2014.926305
  • Thomas-Sunesson, D., Hakuta, K., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 1–10. doi:10.1080/13670050.2016.1148114
  • Tse, C.-S., & Altarriba, J. (2012). The effects of first- and second-language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: Evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 663–676. doi:10.1017/S1366728912000077
  • Voss, M., Chaddock, L., Kim, J., VanPatter, M., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., … Kramer, A. F. (2011). Aerobic fitness is associated with greater efficiency of the network underlying cognitive control in preadolescent children. Neuroscience, 199, 166–176. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.10.009
  • Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., Yang, J., James, B. D., & Bennett, D. A. (2015). Early life instruction in foreign language and music and incidence of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 29, 292–302. doi:10.1037/neu0000129
  • Yow, W. Q., & Li, X. (2015). Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00164
  • Zahodne, L. B., Glymour, M. M., Sparks, C., Bontempo, D., Dixon, R. A., MacDonald, S. W., & Manly, J. J. (2011). Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-year evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 1039–1046. doi:10.1017/S1355617711001044
  • Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M. T., Stern, Y., & Manly, J. J. (2014). Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology, 28, 238–246. doi:10.1037/neu0000014
  • Zhou, B., & Krott, A. (2016). Data trimming procedure can eliminate bilingual cognitive advantage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1221–1230.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.