Publication Cover
Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition
A Journal on Normal and Dysfunctional Development
Volume 30, 2023 - Issue 2
369
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Article

Cognitive decline, socioemotional change, or both? How the science of aging can inform future research on sacrificial moral dilemmas

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 272-299 | Received 09 Apr 2021, Accepted 10 Dec 2021, Published online: 22 Dec 2021

References

  • Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Grilli, M. D., & Irish, M. (2019). A review and reappraisal of the default network in normal aging and dementia. In J. R. Andrews-Hanna, M. D. Grilli, and M. Irish (Eds.), Oxford research encyclopedia of psychology. Oxford University Press, 1–32 . https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.384
  • Bishop, N. A., Lu, T., & Yankner, B. A. (2010). Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature, 464(7288), 529–535. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08983
  • Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(1), 26–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x
  • Brown, C. A., Hakun, J. G., Zhu, Z., Johnson, N. F., & Gold, B. T. (2015). White matter microstructure contributes to age-related declines in task-induced deactivation of the default mode network. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 7 , 194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00194
  • Brown, C. A., Schmitt, F. A., Smith, C. D., & Gold, B. T. (2019). Distinct patterns of default mode and executive control network circuitry contribute to present and future executive function in older adults. NeuroImage, 195 (2019) , 320–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.073
  • Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011
  • Carhart-Harris, R., & Nutt, D. (2017). Serotonin and brain function: A tale of two receptors. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 31(9), 1091–1120. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881117725915
  • Carmona-Perera, M., Reyes Del Paso, G. A., Pérez-García, M., & Verdejo-García, A. (2013). Heart rate correlates of utilitarian moral decision-making in alcoholism. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 133(2), 413–419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.06.023
  • Carmona‐Perera, M., Clark, L., Young, L., Pérez‐García, M., & Verdejo‐García, A. (2014). Impaired decoding of fear and disgust predicts utilitarian moral judgment in alcohol-dependent individuals. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 38(1), 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.12245
  • Carr, A. R., Paholpak, P., Daianu, M., Fong, S. S., Mather, M., Jimenez, E. E., Thompson, P., & Mendez, M. F. (2015). An investigation of care-based vs. rule-based morality in frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, and healthy controls. Neuropsychologia, 78 (1) , 73–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.09.033
  • Carstensen, L. L., Fung, H. H., & Charles, S. T. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion, 27(2), 103–123. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024569803230
  • Chiong, W., Wilson, S. M., D’Esposito, M., Kayser, A. S., Grossman, S. N., Poorzand, P., Seeley, W. W., Miller, B. L., & Rankin, K. P. (2013). The salience network causally influences default mode network activity during moral reasoning. Brain, 136(6), 1929–1941. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt066
  • Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Làdavas, E., & Di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 84–92. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm001
  • Cieri, F., & Esposito, R. (2018). Neuroaging through the lens of the resting state networks. BioMed Research International 2018 , 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/5080981
  • Cools, R. (2008). Role of dopamine in the motivational and cognitive control of behavior. The Neuroscientist, 14(4), 381–395. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858408317009
  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
  • Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits. In F. I. M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 191–211). Springer US. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-4178-9_11
  • Crockett, M. J., Siegel, J. Z., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Ousdal, O. T., Story, G., Frieband, C., Grosse-Rueskamp, J. M., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2015). Dissociable effects of serotonin and dopamine on the valuation of harm in moral decision making. Current Biology, 25(14), 1852–1859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.021
  • Curtis, C. E., & D’Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 415–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9
  • D’Esposito, M. (2007). From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 761–772. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2086
  • Damoiseaux, J. S. (2017). Effects of aging on functional and structural brain connectivity. NeuroImage, 160 (2017) , 32–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.077
  • Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Qué PASA? The posterior–anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201–1209. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm155
  • Dennis, N. A., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In F. I. M. Craik, and T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd (Psychology Press) ed.) (pp. 1–54).
  • Dolcos, F., Rice, H. J., & Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: Right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 819–825. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00068-4
  • Duarte, A., & Kensinger, E. (2019). Age-related changes in episodic memory. In G. Samanez-Larkin (Ed.), The aging brain: Functional adaptation across adulthood (pp. 111–134). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000143-005
  • Ellemers, N., van der Toorn, J., Paunov, Y., & van Leeuwen, T. (2019). The psychology of morality: A review and analysis of empirical studies published from 1940 through 2017. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(4), 332–366. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318811759
  • Erikson, E. H., Erikson, J. M., & Kivnick, H. Q. (1989). Vital involvement in old age. W. W. Norton & Company.
  • Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. W. W. Norton & Company.
  • Fjell, A. M., McEvoy, L., Holland, D., Dale, A. M., & Walhovd, K. B., & Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. (2014). What is normal in normal aging? Effects of aging, amyloid and Alzheimer’s disease on the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus. Progress in Neurobiology, 117 , 20–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.004
  • Fong, S. S., Navarrete, C. D., Perfecto, S. E., Carr, A. R., Jimenez, E. E., & Mendez, M. F. (2017). Behavioral and autonomic reactivity to moral dilemmas in frontotemporal dementia versus Alzheimer’s disease. Social Neuroscience, 12(4), 409–418. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1186111
  • Foot, P. (1967). The problem of abortion and the doctrine of the double effect. Oxford Review, 5, 6–16. https://philpapers.org/archive/footpo-2.pdf
  • Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Raichle, M. E., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(27), 9673–9678. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0504136102
  • Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L. R., & Langdon, P. E. (2018). Moral decision-making and moral development: Toward an integrative framework. Developmental Review, 49, 80–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.06.001
  • Gosling, S. D., Sandy, C. J., John, O. P., & Potter, J. (2010). Wired but not WEIRD: The promise of the internet in reaching more diverse samples. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 94–95. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000300
  • Grady, C. L., Maisog, J. M., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mentis, M. J., Salerno, J. A., Pietrini, P., Wagner, E., & Haxby, J. V. (1994). Age-related changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual processing of faces and location. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(3), 1450–1462. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01450.1994
  • Greene, J. D., Morelli, S. A., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107(3), 1144–1154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004
  • Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389–400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027
  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105–2108. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872
  • Greene, J. D., & Young, L. (2020). The cognitive neuroscience of moral judgment and decision making. In M. S. Gazzaniga Ed., The cognitive neurosciences (Vol. 6). The MIT Press 1–12 .
  • Greene, J. D. (2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 322–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.004
  • Greene, J. D. (2013). Moral tribes: Emotion, reason, and the gap between us and them. Penguin.
  • Greene, J. D. (2008). The secret joke of Kant’s soul. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral psychology: The neuroscience of morality (pp. 35–79). MIT Press.
  • Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(1), 253–258. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0135058100
  • Gutchess, A. H., Sokal, R., Coleman, J. A., Gotthilf, G., Grewal, L., & Rosa, N. (2015). Age differences in self-referencing: Evidence for common and distinct encoding strategies. Brain Research, 1612 (2015) , 118–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.033
  • Han, H. (2017). Neural correlates of moral sensitivity and moral judgment associated with brain circuitries of selfhood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Moral Education, 46(2), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2016.1262834
  • Hannikainen, I. R., Machery, E., & Cushman, F. A. (2018). Is utilitarian sacrifice becoming more morally permissible? Cognition, 170 (2018) , 95–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.013
  • Harris, C. B., Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2014). The functions of autobiographical memory: An integrative approach. Memory, 22(5), 559–581. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.806555
  • Hartshorne, J. K., & Germine, L. T. (2015). When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science, 26(4), 433–443. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614567339
  • Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., & Rypma, B. (1991). Age and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(1), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.17.1.163
  • Henninger, D. E., Madden, D. J., & Huettel, S. A. (2010). Processing speed and memory mediate age-related differences in decision making. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 262–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019096
  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
  • Huang, S., Faul, L., Sevinc, G., Mwilambwe-Tshilobo, L., Setton, R., Lockrow, A. W., Ebner, N. C., Turner, G. R., Spreng, R. N., & De Brigard, F. (2021). Age differences in intuitive moral decision-making: Associations with inter-network neural connectivity. Psychology and Aging, 36(8), 902–916. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000633
  • Jeurissen, D., Sack, A. T., Roebroeck, A., Russ, B. E., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2014). TMS affects moral judgment, showing the role of DLPFC and TPJ in cognitive and emotional processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 1-9 / Article 18 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2014.00018
  • Kahane, G. (2015). Sidetracked by trolleys: Why sacrificial moral dilemmas tell us little (or nothing) about utilitarian judgment. Social Neuroscience, 10(5), 551–560. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1023400
  • Karrer, T. M., Josef, A. K., Mata, R., Morris, E. D., & Samanez-Larkin, G. R. (2017). Reduced dopamine receptors and transporters but not synthesis capacity in normal aging adults: A meta-analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, 57 (2017) , 36–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.006
  • Karrer, T. M., McLaughlin, C. L., Guaglianone, C. P., & Samanez-Larkin, G. R. (2019). Reduced serotonin receptors and transporters in normal aging adults: A meta-analysis of PET and SPECT imaging studies. Neurobiology of Aging, 80 (2019) , 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.021
  • Kensinger, E. A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2017). Cognitive aging in a social and affective context: Advances over the past 50 years. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 72(1), 61–70. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw056
  • Kensinger, E. A., & Gutchess, A. (2015). Memory for emotional and social information in adulthood and old age. In D. R. Addis, M. Barense, & A. Duarte (Eds.), The wiley handbook on the cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp. 393–414). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118332634.ch19
  • Koechlin, E., Ody, C., & Kouneiher, F. (2003). The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Science, 302(5648), 1181–1185. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1088545
  • Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446(7138), 908–911. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05631
  • Kuehne, M., Heimrath, K., Heinze, H.-J., Zaehle, T., & Zalla, T. (2015). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex shifts preference of moral judgments. PLOS ONE, 10(5), e0127061. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0127061
  • Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time Counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 125–139. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.125
  • Lemaitre, H., Goldman, A. L., Sambataro, F., Verchinski, B. A., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Weinberger, D. R., & Mattay, V. S. (2012). Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: Nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume? Neurobiology of Aging, 33(3), 617.e1–617.e9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.013
  • Lighthall, N. R. (2020). Neural mechanisms of decision-making in aging. WIREs Cognitive Science, 11(1), e1519. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1519
  • Luong, G., Charles, S. T., & Fingerman, K. L. (2011). Better with age: Social relationships across adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 28(1), 9–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407510391362
  • MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, A. V., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
  • MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: A dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 598–609. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.598
  • Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 496–502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005
  • Maxfield, M., Kluck, B., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Cox, C. R., Solomon, S., & Weise, D. (2007). Age-related differences in responses to thoughts of one’s own death: Mortality salience and judgments of moral transgressions. Psychology and Aging, 22(2), 341–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.22.2.341
  • Maxfield, M., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Pepin, R., & Davis, H. P. (2012). The moderating role of executive functioning in older adults’ responses to a reminder of mortality. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 256–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023902
  • McNair, S., Okan, Y., Hadjichristidis, C., & Bruin, W. B. D. (2019). Age differences in moral judgment: Older adults are more deontological than younger adults. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 32(1), 47–60. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2086
  • Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 655–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0
  • Mill, J. S. (1895). Utilitarianism (Vol. 3). Routledge.
  • Moll, J., & de Oliveira-souza, R. (2007). Moral judgments, emotions and the utilitarian brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 319–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.001
  • Moore, A. B., Clark, B. A., & Kane, M. J. (2008). Who shalt not kill? Individual differences in working memory capacity, executive control, and moral Judgment. Psychological Science, 19(6), 549–557. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02122.x
  • Nashiro, K., Sakaki, M., Braskie, M. N., & Mather, M. (2017). Resting-state networks associated with cognitive processing show more age-related decline than those associated with emotional processing. Neurobiology of Aging, 54 (2017) , 152–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.03.003
  • Ogletree, A. M., & Katz, B. (2020). How do older adults recruited using mTurk differ from those in a national probability sample? The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 93 (2) , 700–721. https://doi.org/10.1177/0091415020940197
  • Park, D. C., Polk, T. A., Park, R., Minear, M., Savage, A., & Smith, M. R. (2004). Aging reduces neural specialization in ventral visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(35), 13091–13095. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0405148101
  • Park, D. C. (2000). The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. In D. C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 3–21). Psychology Press.
  • Park, G., Kappes, A., Rho, Y., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2016). At the heart of morality lies neuro-visceral integration: Lower cardiac vagal tone predicts utilitarian moral judgment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(10), 1588–1596. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw077
  • Persson, J., Lustig, C., Nelson, J. K., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2007). Age Differences in Deactivation: A Link to Cognitive Control? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(6), 1021–1032. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.1021
  • Ponsi, G., Scattolin, M., Villa, R., & Aglioti, S. M. (2021). Human moral decision-making through the lens of Parkinson’s disease. Npj Parkinson’s Disease, 7(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41531-021-00167-w
  • Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2015). Thirty years of terror management theory: From genesis to revelation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 1–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.03.001
  • Qin, P., & Northoff, G. (2011). How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network? NeuroImage, 57(3), 1221–1233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.028
  • Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676–682. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676
  • Raz, N. (1997). Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: Differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cerebral Cortex, 7(3), 268–282. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/7.3.268
  • Reed, A. E., & Carstensen, L. L. (2012). The theory behind the age-related positivity effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1-9 / Article 339 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00339
  • Reniers, R. L. E. P., Corcoran, R., Völlm, B. A., Mashru, A., Howard, R., & Liddle, P. F. (2012). Moral decision-making, ToM, empathy and the default mode network. Biological Psychology, 90(3), 202–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.009
  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177–182. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x
  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: A new look at old problems. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65B(4), 405–415. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbq035
  • Rosen, J. B., Brand, M., & Kalbe, E. (2016). Empathy mediates the effects of age and sex on altruistic moral decision making. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 1-16 / Article 67. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00067
  • Rosen, J. B., Rott, E., Ebersbach, G., & Kalbe, E. (2015). Altered moral decision-making in patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 21(10), 1191–1199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.08.016
  • Rutt, J. L., & Löckenhoff, C. E. (2016). From past to future: Temporal self-continuity across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 31(6), 631–639. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000090
  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403
  • Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., Reiss, A. L., & Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2349–2356. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007
  • Sevinc, G., Gurvit, H., & Spreng, R. N. (2017). Salience network engagement with the detection of morally laden information. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 1118–1127. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx035
  • Sevinc, G., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). Contextual and perceptual brain processes underlying moral cognition: A quantitative meta-analysis of moral reasoning and moral emotions. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e87427. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087427
  • Siegel, J. Z., & Crockett, M. J. (2013). How serotonin shapes moral judgment and behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1299(1), 42–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12229
  • Spreng, R. N., Lockrow, A. W., DuPre, E., Setton, R., Spreng, K. A. P., & Turner, G. R. (2018). Semanticized autobiographical memory and the default – Executive coupling hypothesis of aging. Neuropsychologia, 110 (2018) , 37–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.009
  • Spreng, R. N., & Turner, G. R. (2019). The shifting architecture of cognition and brain function in older adulthood. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(4), 523–542. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619827511
  • Strohminger, N., Lewis, R. L., & Meyer, D. E. (2011). Divergent effects of different positive emotions on moral judgment. Cognition, 119(2), 295–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.012
  • Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2017). Aging enhances cognitive biases to friends but not the self. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(6), 2021–2030. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1264-1
  • Thomson, J. J. (1986). Rights, restitution, and risk: Essays, in moral theory. Harvard University Press.
  • Trémolière, B., De Neys, W., & Bonnefon, J.-F. (2014). The grim reasoner: Analytical reasoning under mortality salience. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(3), 333–351. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2013.823888
  • Trémolière, B., Neys, W. D., & Bonnefon, J.-F. (2012). Mortality salience and morality: Thinking about death makes people less utilitarian. Cognition, 124(3), 379–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.011
  • Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science, 17(6), 476–477. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01731.x
  • Yates, J. F., & de Oliveira, S. (2016). Culture and decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 136 (2016) , 106–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.05.003
  • Yee, D. M., & Braver, T. S. (2018). Interactions of motivation and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 83–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.11.009
  • Zheng, H., Lu, X., & Huang, D. (2018). TDCS over DLPFC leads to less utilitarian response in moral-personal judgment. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 1-8 / Article 193 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00193

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.