777
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Moral Clarity in Narratives Elicits Greater Cooperation than Moral Ambiguity

, , , &

REFERENCES

  • Ashby, M, & Carroll, B. (2014). David Foster Wallace was right: Irony is ruining our culture. Salon. Retrieved from http://www.salon.com/2014/04/13/david_foster_wallace_ as_right_irony_is_ruining_our_culture/
  • Balliet, D., & Joireman, J. (2010). Ego depletion reduces proselfs’ concern with the well-being of others. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13, 227–239.
  • Barnhoorn, J. S., Haasnoot, E., Bocanegra, B. R., & van Steenbergen, H. (2014). QRTEngine: An easy solution for running online reaction time experiments using Qualtrics. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 918–929. doi:10.3758/s13428-014-0530-7
  • Bloom, P. (2010). How do morals change? Nature, 464, 490. doi:10.1038/464490a
  • Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition advancing the debate. Perspectives on psychological science, 8, 223–241.
  • Goodwin, B. C. (1972). Temporal order as the origin of spatial order in embryos. In J. T. Fraser, F. C. Haber, & G. H. Muller, The study of time (pp. 190–199). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
  • Goodwin, B. C, & Cohen, M. H. (1969). A phase-shift model for the spatial and temporal organization of developing systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 1, 323–334.
  • Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1985). A valid procedure for obtaining self-report of affect in marital interaction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 151–160. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.53.2.151
  • Grahn, J. A., & McAuley, J. D. (2009). Neural bases of individual differences in beat perception. NeuroImage, 47, 1894–1903. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.039
  • Grizzard, M. (2014, November 17). The moral sandbox of media entertainment. University at Buffalo, Communication Colloquium, Buffalo, NY.
  • Grizzard, M., Tamborini, R., Lewis, R. J., Wang, L., & Prabhu, S. (2014). Being bad in a video game can make us morally sensitive. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 499–504. doi:10.1089/cyber.2013.0658.
  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological review, 108(4), 814–834. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814
  • Haidt, J., Seder, J. P., & Kesebir, S. (2008). Hive psychology, happiness, and public policy. The Journal of Legal Studies, 37, 133–156. doi:10.7208/chicago/9780226676029.003.0006
  • Hasson, U., Landesman, O., Knappmeyer, B., Vallines, I., Rubin, N., & Heeger, D. J. (2008). Neurocinematics: The neuroscience of film. Projections, 2, 1–26. doi:10.3167/proj.2008.020102
  • Iberall, A. S., & McCulloch, W. S. (1969). The organizing principle of complex living systems. Journal of Basic Engineering, 91, 290–294.
  • Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83, 323–355. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.83.5.323
  • Kimberly, R. P. (1970). Rhythmic patterns in human interaction. Nature, 22, 88–90. doi:10.1038/228088a0
  • Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 354–364. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004
  • Krakowiak, K. M., & Oliver, M. B. (2012). When good characters do bad things: Examining the effect of moral ambiguity on enjoyment. Journal of Communication, 62, 117–135.
  • Lang, A. (2014). Dynamic human-centered communication systems theory. The Information Society, 30, 60–70. doi:10.1080/01972243.2013.856364
  • Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.119
  • Lester, B. M., Hoffman, J., & Brazelton, T. B. (1985). The rhythmic structure of mother-infant interaction in term and preterm infants. Child Development, 15–27. doi:10.2307/1130169
  • Lewis, R. J. Tamborini, R., & Weber, R. (2014). Testing a dual-process model of media enjoyment and appreciation. Journal of Communication, 64, 397–416. doi:10.1111/jcom.12101
  • Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., & Schmidt, R. C. (2009). Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 320–339. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01022.x
  • McAuley, J.D., Jones, M. R., Holub S., Johnston, H. M. and Miller, N. S. (2006). The time of our lives: Lifespan development of timing and event tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 135, 348–367. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348
  • Meier, M. E., & Kane, M. J. (2013). Working memory capacity and Stroop interference: Global versus local indices of executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 748–759.
  • Miller, J. E., Carlson, L. A., & McAuley, J. D. (2012). When what you hear influences when you see: Listening to an auditory rhythm influences the temporal allocation of visual attention. Psychological Science, 24, 11–18. doi:10.1177/0956797612446707.
  • Oliver, M. B., & Bartsch, A. (2010). Appreciation as audience response: Exploring entertainment gratifications beyond hedonism. Human Communication Research, 36, 53–81. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01368.x
  • Paukner, A., Suomi, S. J., Visalberghi, E., & Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. Science, 325, 880–883. doi:10.1126/science.1176269
  • Piaget, J. (1932). Le jugement moral chez l’enfant [Moral judgment of the child]. Paris, France: F. Alcan.
  • Raney, A. A. (2004). Expanding disposition theory: Reconsidering character liking, moral evaluations, and enjoyment. Communication Theory, 14, 348–369.
  • Sharpe, L. (2006). Württemberg and ‘Die Räuber’. In L. Sharpe (Ed.), Friedrich Schiller: Drama, thought and politics (pp. 6–30). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Strogatz, S. (2003). Sync: The emerging science of spontaneous order. New York, NY: Hyperion.
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of experimental psychology, 18, 643–662.
  • Tamborini, R. (2011). Moral intuition and media entertainment. Journal of Media Psychology, 23, 39–45.
  • Tamborini, R. (2012). A model of intuitive morality and exemplars. In R. Tamborini (Ed.), Media and the moral mind (pp. 43–74). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Tamborini, R., Grizzard, M. N., Eden, A., & Lewis, R. J. (2011, November). Imperfect heroes and villains: Patterns of upholding and violating distinct moral domains and character appeal. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, New Orleans, LA.
  • Tronick, E. D., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1977). Mutuality in mother‐infant interaction. Journal of Communication, 27(2), 74–79. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1977.tb01829.x
  • Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11, 262–266. doi:10.1037/a0021302
  • Van Lange, P. A., Bekkers, R., Chirumbolo, A., & Leone, L. (2012). Are conservatives less likely to be prosocial than liberals? From games to ideology, political preferences and voting. European Journal of Personality, 26, 461–473.
  • Warner, R. (1996). Coordinated cycles in behavior and physiology during face-to-face social interactions. In J. H. Watt & C. A. VanLear (Eds.), Dynamic patterns in communication processes (pp. 327–352). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Warner, R. M. (1992). Cyclicity of vocal activity increases during conversation: Support for a nonlinear systems model of dyadic social interaction. Behavioral Science, 37, 128–138. doi:10.1002/bs.3830370204
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
  • Weber, R., Eden, A., & Mathiak K. (2011, June). Seeing bad people punished makes us think alike: Social norm violations in television drama elicit cortical synchronization in viewers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association (ICA), Boston, MA.
  • Weber, R., & Mangus, M. (2014). Can morally–salient narratives foster cooperation? Exploring the prosocial effects of synchronous group activity. Unpublished manuscript.
  • Weber, R., Popova, L., & Mangus, J. M. (2012). Universal morality, mediated narratives, and neural synchrony. In R.Tamborini (Ed.), Media and the moral mind (pp. 26–42). New York, NY: Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780203127070
  • Weber, R. A., Camerer, C. F., & Knez, M. (2004). Timing and virtual observability in ultimatum bargaining and “weak link” coordination games. Experimental Economics, 7, 25–48. doi:10.1023/A:1026257921046
  • Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20, 1–5. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x
  • Yamagishi, T., Kikuchi, M., & Kosugi, M. (1999). Trust, gullibility, and social intelligence. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 145–161. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00030

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.