322
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Article

Savvy or Haphazard? Comparing Preschoolers’ Performance Across Selective Learning Tasks Based on Different Epistemic Indicators

References

  • Bascandziev, I., & Harris, P. L. (2016). The beautiful and the accurate: Are children’s selective trust decisions biased? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 92–105. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.017
  • Bernard, S., Castelain, T., Mercier, H., Kaufmann, L., Van der Henst, J. B., & Clément, F. (2016). The boss is always right: Preschoolers endorse the testimony of a dominant over that of a subordinate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 307–317. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.007
  • Birch, S. A. J., Akmal, N., & Frampton, K. L. (2010). Two-year-olds are vigilant of others’ non-verbal cues to credibility. Developmental Science, 13(2), 363–369. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00906.x
  • Birch, S. A. J., Vauthier, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2008). Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition, 107(3), 1018–1034. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008
  • Brooker, I., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2013). Is a bird an apple? The effect of speaker labeling accuracy on infants’ word learning, imitation, and helping behaviors. Infancy, 18(SUPPL.1), 46–69. doi:10.1111/infa.12027
  • Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Birch, S. A. J. (2011). Epistemic states and traits: Preschoolers appreciate the differential informativeness of situation-specific and person-specific cues to knowledge. Child Development, 82(6), 1788–1796. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01662.x
  • Brosseau-Liard, P. E., Penney, D., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2015). Theory of mind selectively predicts preschoolers’ knowledge-based selective word learning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 464–475. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12107
  • Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2014). Sensitivity to confidence cues increases during the second year of life. Infancy, 19(5), 461–475. doi:10.1111/infa.12056
  • Canfield, C. F., Saudino, K. J., & Ganea, P. A. (2015). The role of temperament in children’s reliance on others as sources of information. Infant and Child Development, 24, 435–451. doi:10.1002/icd.1892
  • Chudek, M., Birch, S. A. J., Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Henrich, J. (2013). Culture-gene coevolutionary theory and children’s selective social learning. In M. R. Banaji & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 181–210). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Corriveau, K., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Developmental Science, 12(3), 426–437. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00792.x
  • Corriveau, K. H., Kurkul, K., & Arunachalam, S. (2016). Preschoolers’ preference for syntactic complexity varies by socioeconomic status. Child Development, 87(5), 1529–1537. doi:10.1111/cdev.12553
  • Crivello, C., Phillips, S., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2017). Selective social learning in infancy : Looking for mechanisms. Developmental Science, (June). doi:10.1111/desc.12592
  • DiYanni, C., Nini, D., Rheel, W., & Livelli, A. (2012). “I won”t trust you if i think you’re trying to deceive Me’: Relations between selective trust, theory of mind, and imitation in early childhood. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(3), 354–371. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.590462
  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). PPVT-4: Peabody picture vocabulary test. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.
  • Einav, S., & Robinson, E. J. (2010). Children’s sensitivity to error magnitude when evaluating informants. Cognitive Development, 25(3), 218–232. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.04.002
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi:10.3758/bf03193146
  • Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/2-7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X
  • Harris, P. L., Koenig, M. A., Corriveau, K. H., & Jaswal, V. K. (2018). Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(1), annurev-psych-122216-011710. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011710
  • Henderson, A. M. E., Graham, S. A., & Schell, V. (2015). 24-month-olds’ selective learning is not an all-or-none phenomenon. PLoS ONE, 10(6), 1–16. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0131215
  • Henderson, A. M. E., Sabbagh, M. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2013). Preschoolers’ selective learning is guided by the principle of relevance. Cognition, 126(2), 246–257. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.006
  • Hermes, J., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). The development of selective trust: Prospects for a dual-process account. Child Development Perspectives, 1–5. doi:10.1111/cdep.12274
  • Heyman, G. D., Sritanyaratana, L., & Vanderbilt, K. E. (2013). Young children’s trust in overtly misleading advice. Cognitive Science, 37(4), 646–667. doi:10.1111/cogs.12020
  • Hübscher, I., Esteve-Gibert, N., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P. (2017). Intonation and gesture as bootstrapping devices in speaker uncertainty. First Language, 37(1), 24–41. doi:10.1177/0142723716673953
  • Jaswal, V. K., & Kondrad, R. L. (2016). Why children are not always epistemically vigilant: Cognitive limits and social considerations. Child Development Perspectives, 10(4), 240–244. doi:10.1111/cdep.12187
  • Jaswal, V. K., & Malone, L. S. (2007). Turning believers into skeptics: 3-year-olds’ sensitivity to cues to speaker credibility. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(3), 263–283. doi:10.1080/15248370701446392
  • Jaswal, V. K., & Neely, L. A. (2006). Adults don’t always know best preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17(9), 757–758. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x
  • Jaswal, V. K., Perez-Edgar, K., Kondrad, R. L., Palmquist, C. M., Cole, C. A., & Cole, C. E. (2014). Can’t stop believing: Inhibitory control and resistance to misleading testimony. Developmental Science, 17(6), 965–976. doi:10.1111/desc.12187
  • Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005a). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76(6), 1261–1277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
  • Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005b). The role of social cognition in early trust. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 457–459. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.006
  • Koenig, M. A., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2013). Selective social learning: New perspectives on learning from others. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 399–403. doi:10.1037/a0031619
  • Krogh-Jespersen, S., & Echols, C. H. (2012). The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. Child Development, 83, 581–590. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01713.x
  • Krogh-Jespersen, S., & Echols, C. H. (2018). Children’s explicit assessments of reliability influence their willingness to learn novel labels. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 170, 197–206. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.007
  • Landrum, A. R., Mills, C. M., & Johnston, A. M. (2013). When do children trust the expert? Benevolence information influences children’s trust more than expertise. Developmental Science, 16(4), 622–638. doi:10.1111/desc.12059
  • Lord, F. M., & Novick, M. (1968). Statistical theories of mental test scores. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Lucas, A. J., & Lewis, C. (2010). Should we trust experiments on trust? Human Development, 53(4), 167–172. doi:10.1159/000320044
  • Lucas, A. J., Lewis, C., Pala, F. C., Wong, K., & Berridge, D. (2013). Social-cognitive processes in preschoolers’ selective trust: Three cultures compared. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 579–590. doi:10.1037/a0029864
  • Matsui, T., Yamamoto, T., Miura, Y., & McCagg, P. (2016). Young children’s early sensitivity to linguistic indications of speaker certainty in their selective word learning. Lingua, 175–176, 83–96. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2015.10.007
  • Mills, C. M. (2013). Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 404–418. doi:10.1037/a0029500
  • Nurmsoo, E., & Robinson, E. J. (2009). Identifying unreliable informants: Do children excuse past inaccuracy? Developmental Science, 12(1), 41–47. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00750.x
  • Nurmsoo, E., Robinson, E. J., & Butterfill, S. A. (2010). Children’s selective learning from others. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1(4), 551–561. doi:10.1007/s13164-010-0043-y
  • O’Neill, D. K., Astington, J. W., & Flavell, J. H. (1992). Young children’s understanding of the role that sensory experiences play in knowledge acquisition. Child Development, 63(2), 474–490. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01641.x
  • Pasquini, E. S., Corriveau, K. H., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2007). Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1216–1226. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216
  • Pillow, B. H., & Weed, S. T. (1997). Preschool children’s use of information about age and perceptual access to infer another person’s knowledge. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 158(3), 365–376. doi:10.1080/00221329709596675
  • Poulin-Dubois, D., & Brosseau-Liard, P. (2016). The developmental origins of selective social learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(1), 60–64. doi:10.1177/0963721415613962
  • Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the children’s behavior questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(1), 102–112. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09
  • Reifen Tagar, M., Federico, C. M., Lyons, K. E., Ludeke, S., & Koenig, M. A. (2014). Heralding the authoritarian? Orientation toward authority in early childhood. Psychological Science, 25(4), 883–892. doi:10.1177/0956797613516470
  • Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 354–373. doi:10.1037/a0029315
  • Robinson, E. J., Haigh, S. N., & Pendle, J. E. C. (2008). Children’s working understanding of the knowledge gained from seeing and feeling. Developmental Science, 11(2), 299–305. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00676.x
  • Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between Preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72(4), 1054–1070. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00334
  • Shafto, P., Eaves, B., Navarro, D. J., & Perfors, A. (2012). Epistemic trust: Modeling children’s reasoning about others’ knowledge and intent. Developmental Science, 15(3), 436–447. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01135.x
  • Sobel, D. M., & Kushnir, T. (2013). Knowledge matters: How children evaluate the reliability of testimony as a process of rational inference. Psychological Review, 120(4), 779–797. doi:10.1037/a0034191
  • Tahiroglu, D., Moses, L. J., Carlson, S. M., Mahy, C. E. V., Olofson, E. L., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2014). The Children’s Social Understanding Scale: Construction and validation of a parent-report measure for assessing individual differences in children’s theories of mind. Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2485–2497. doi:10.1037/a0037914
  • Tummeltshammer, K. S., Wu, R., Sobel, D. M., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2014). Infants track the reliability of potential informants. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1730–1738. doi:10.1177/0956797614540178
  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. doi:10.1111/cdev.2004.75.issue-2

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.