1,109
Views
21
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Article

Children’s Understanding and Use of Four Dimensions of Social Status

References

  • Ahl, R. E., & Dunham, Y. (2019). “Wealth makes many friends”: Children expect more giving from resource‐rich than resource‐poor individuals. Child Development, 90(2), 524–543. doi:10.1111/cdev.12922
  • Bernard, S., Castelain, T., Mercier, H., Kaufmann, L., Van der Henst, J. B., & Clément, F. (2016). The boss is always right: Preschoolers endorse the testimony of a dominant over that of a subordinate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 152, 307–317. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.007
  • Bickman, L. (1971). The effect of social status on the honesty of others. The Journal of Social Psychology, 85(1), 87–92. doi:10.1080/00224545.1971.9918547
  • Borke, H. (1973). The development of empathy in Chinese and American children between three and six years of age: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 9(1), 102–108. doi:10.1037/h0035080
  • Brey, E., & Shutts, K. (2015). Children use nonverbal cues to make inferences about social power. Child Development, 86(1), 276–286. doi:10.1111/cdev.12334
  • Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. New York, USA: McGraw-Hill.
  • Cain, K. M., Heyman, G. D., & Walker, M. E. (1997). Preschoolers’ ability to make dispositional predictions within and across domain. Social Development, 6(1), 53–75. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00094.x
  • Castelain, T., Bernard, S., Van der Henst, J. B., & Mercier, H. (2016). The influence of power and reason on young Maya children’s endorsement of testimony. Developmental Science, 19(6), 957–966. doi:10.1111/desc.12336
  • Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Clément, F., Kaufmann, L., Berchtold, A., Reboul, A., & Van der Henst, J. B. (2015). How preschoolers use cues of dominance to make sense of their social environment. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(4), 587–607. doi:10.1080/15248372.2014.926269
  • Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Clément, F., Kaufmann, L., Reboul, A., & Van der Henst, J. B. (2016). Children’s allocation of resources in social dominance situations. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1843–1857. doi:10.1037/dev0000164
  • Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S., & Henrich, J. (2012). Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(1), 46–56. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.005
  • Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82(3), 793–811. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x
  • Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Sadovsky, A. (2006). Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 517–549). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Elenbaas, L., & Killen, M. (2016). Children rectify inequalities for disadvantaged groups. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1318–1329. doi:10.1037/dev0000154
  • Elenbaas, L., Rizzo, M. T., Cooley, S., & Killen, M. (2016). Rectifying social inequalities in a resource allocation task. Cognition, 155, 176–187. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.002
  • Enright, E. A., Gweon, H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2017). ‘To the victor go the spoils’: Infants expect resources to align with dominance structures. Cognition, 164, 8–21. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.008
  • Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenback, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454(7208), 1079–1083. doi:10.1038/nature07155
  • Gazes, R. P., Hampton, R. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2017). Transitive inference of social dominance by human infants. Developmental Science, 20(2), e12367. doi:10.1111/desc.12367
  • Gelfand, D. M., Hartmann, D. P., Cromer, C. C., Smith, C. L., & Page, B. C. (1975). The effects of instructional prompts and praise on children’s donation rates. Child Development, 46(4), 980–983. doi:10.2307/1128408
  • Gülgöz, S., & Gelman, S. A. (2017). Who’s the boss? Concepts of social power across development. Child Development, 88(3), 946–963. doi:10.1111/cdev.12643
  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00071-4
  • Højsgaard, S., Halekoh, U., & Yan, J. (2006). The R Package geepack for generalized estimating equations. Journal of Statistical Software, 15(2), 1–11.
  • Horwitz, S. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2017). The rich—love them or hate them? Divergent implicit and explicit attitudes toward the wealthy. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20(1), 3–31. doi:10.1177/1368430215596075
  • Horwitz, S. R., Shutts, K., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Social class differences produce social group preferences. Developmental Science, 17(6), 991–1002. doi:10.1111/desc.12181
  • Johnson, M. K., Kim, J. K., & Risse, G. (1985). Do alcoholic Korsakoff’s syndrome patients acquire affective reactions? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(1), 22–36. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.11.1.22
  • Kajanus, A., Afshordi, N., & Warneken, F. (2020). Children’s understanding of dominance and prestige in China and the UK. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(1), 23–34. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.08.002
  • Kenward, B., & Dahl, M. (2011). Preschoolers distribute scarce resources according to the moral valence of recipients’ previous actions. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1054–1064. doi:10.1037/a0023869
  • Koski, J. E., Xie, H., & Olson, I. R. (2015). Understanding social hierarchies: The neural and psychological foundations of status perception. Social Neuroscience, 10(5), 527–550. doi:10.1080/17470919.2015.1013223
  • Lamm, H., & Schwinger, T. (1980). Norms concerning distributive justice: Are needs taken into consideration in allocation decisions? Social Psychology Quarterly, 43(4), 425–429. doi:10.2307/3033962
  • Li, V., Spitzer, B., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Preschoolers reduce inequality while favoring individuals with more. Child Development, 85(3), 1123–1133. doi:10.1111/cdev.12198
  • Lott, B. (2012). The social psychology of class and classism. American Psychologist, 67(8), 650–658. doi:10.1037/a0029369
  • Lourenco, S. F., Bonny, J. W., & Schwartz, B. L. (2016). Children and adults use physical size and numerical alliances in third-party judgments of dominance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2050. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02050
  • Mandalaywala, T. M., Rhodes, M., & Tai, C. (2019). Children’s use of race and gender as cues to social status. PsyArXiv. doi:10.31234/osf.io/3rnej
  • Margoni, F., Baillargeon, R., & Surian, L. (2018). Infants distinguish between leaders and bullies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(38), 8835–8843. doi:10.1073/pnas.1801677115
  • Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2012). Representation of stable social dominance relations by human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(18), 6862–6867. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113194109
  • Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2014). Human infants’ learning of social structures: The case of dominance hierarchy. Psychological Science, 25(1), 250–255. doi:10.1177/0956797613500509
  • Mast, M. S., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 145–165. doi:10.1023/B:JONB.0000039647.94190.21
  • Mookherjee, H. N., & Hogan, H. W. (1981). Class consciousness among young rural children. The Journal of Social Psychology, 114(1), 91–98. doi:10.1080/00224545.1981.9922730
  • Moore, C. (2009). Fairness in children’s resource allocation depends on the recipient. Psychological Science, 20(8), 944–948. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02378.x
  • Moore, D. A. (2016). Preregister if you want to. American Psychologist, 71(3), 238–239. doi:10.1037/a0040195
  • Olson, K. R., Banaji, M. R., Dweck, C. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2006). Children’s biased evaluations of lucky versus unlucky people and their social groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 845–846. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01792.x
  • Olson, K. R., Dunham, Y., Dweck, C. S., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). Judgments of the lucky across development and culture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 757–776. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.757
  • Olson, K. R., Dweck, C. S., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2011). Children’s responses to group-based inequalities: Perpetuation and rectification. Social Cognition, 29(3), 270–287. doi:10.1521/soco.2011.29.3.270
  • Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition, 108(1), 222-231. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003
  • Paulus, M. (2014). The early origins of human charity: Developmental changes in preschoolers’ sharing with poor and wealthy individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 344. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00344
  • Paulus, M. (2016). Friendship trumps neediness: The impact of social relations and others’ wealth on preschool children’s sharing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 106–120. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.02.001
  • Pun, A., Birch, S. A., & Baron, A. S. (2016). Infants use relative numerical group size to infer social dominance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(9), 2376–2381. doi:10.1073/pnas.1514879113
  • Ricard, M., & Kamberk-Kilicci, M. (1995). Children’s empathic responses to emotional complexity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18(2), 211–225. doi:10.1177/016502549501800202
  • Rizzo, M. T., Elenbaas, L., Cooley, S., & Killen, M. (2016). Children’s recognition of fairness and others’ welfare in a resource allocation task: Age related changes. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1307–1317. doi:10.1037/dev0000134
  • Rochat, P., & Robbins, E. (2016). Sharing and fairness in development. In J. Kiverstein (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of philosophy of the social mind (pp. 222–243). New York, USA: Routledge.
  • Roth-Hanania, R., Davidov, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2011). Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: Early signs of concern for others. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(3), 447–458. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.007
  • Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2009). Knowing who’s boss: Implicit perceptions of status from the nonverbal expression of pride. Emotion, 9(5), 631–639. doi:10.1037/a0017089
  • Shutts, K., Brey, E. L., Dornbusch, L. A., Slywotzky, N., & Olson, K. R. (2016). Children use wealth cues to evaluate others. PloS ONE, 11(3), e0149360. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0149360
  • Sigelman, C. K. (2013). Age differences in perceptions of rich and poor people: Is it skill or luck? Social Development, 22(1), 1–18. doi:10.1111/sode.12000
  • Smith, P. K., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). The nonconscious nature of power: Cues and consequences. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(10), 918–938. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00300.x
  • Terrizzi, B. F., Brey, E., Shutts, K., & Beier, J. S. (2019). Children’s developing judgments about the physical manifestations of power. Developmental Psychology, 55(4), 793–808. doi:10.1037/dev0000657
  • Thomas, A. J., Thomsen, L., Lukowski, A. F., Abramyan, M., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2018). Toddlers prefer those who win but not when they win by force. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(9), 662–669. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0415-3
  • Thomsen, L. (2019). The developmental origins of social hierarchy: How infants and young children mentally represent and respond to power and status. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 201–208. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.044
  • Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., & Carey, S. (2011). Big and mighty: Preverbal infants mentally represent social dominance. Science, 331(6016), 477–480. doi:10.1126/science.1199198
  • Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1993). The covert learning of affective valence does not require structures in hippocampal system or amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 79–88. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.79
  • Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 534–543. doi:10.1037/a0014322
  • Van Berkel, L., Crandall, C. S., Eidelman, S., & Blanchar, J. C. (2015). Hierarchy, dominance, and deliberation: Egalitarian values require mental effort. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(9), 1207–1222. doi:10.1177/0146167215591961
  • Van Vugt, M., & Tybur, J. M. (2015). The evolutionary foundations of hierarchy: Status, dominance, prestige, and leadership. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 788–809). New Jersey, USA: Wiley.
  • Van’t Veer, A. E., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2016). Pre-registration in social psychology—a discussion and suggested template. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 2–12. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2016.03.004
  • Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 338–350. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.002
  • Yu, J., Zhu, L., & Leslie, A. M. (2016). Children’s sharing behavior in mini-dictator games: The role of in-group favoritism and theory of mind. Child Development, 87(6), 1747–1757. doi:10.1111/cdev.12635
  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 126–136. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.126

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.