838
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Article

Bilingual Infants are More Sensitive to Morally Relevant Social Behavior than Monolingual Infants

References

  • Barker, R. M., & Bialystok, E. (2019). Processing differences between monolingual and bilingual young adults on an emotion n-back task. Brain and Cognition, 134, 29–43. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.004
  • Barr, R., Rusnak, S. N., Brito, N. H., & Nugent, C. (2019). Actions speak louder than words: Differences in memory flexibility between monolingual and bilingual 18‐month‐olds. Developmental Science, e12881. doi:10.1111/desc.12881
  • Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development (3rd ed., pp. 180–190). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt assessment.
  • Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 93–105. doi:10.1037/a0015466
  • Bialystok, E., & Senman, L. (2004). Executive processes in appearance-reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Development, 75(2), 562–579. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00693.x
  • Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition, 65(1), 33–69. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00040-1
  • Brito, N., & Barr, R. (2012). Influence of bilingualism on memory generalization during infancy. Developmental Science, 15(6), 812–816. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.1184.x
  • Brito, N., & Barr, R. (2014). Flexible memory retrieval in bilingual 6‐month‐old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(5), 1156–1163. doi:10.1002/dev.21188
  • Brojde, C., Ahmed, S., & Colunga, E. (2012). Bilingual and monolingual children attend to different cues when learning new words. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 155. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00155
  • Buac, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2019). Predictors of theory of mind performance in bilingual and monolingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism. doi:10.1177/1367006919826866
  • Dawkins, M. B., Sloane, S., & Baillargeon, R. (2019). Do infants in the first year of life expect equal resource allocations? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 116. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.0011
  • Byers‐Heinlein, K., Behrend, D. A., Said, L. M., Girgis, H., & Poulin‐Dubois, D. (2017). Monolingual and bilingual children’s social preferences for monolingual and bilingual speakers. Developmental Science, 20(4), e12392. doi:10.1111/desc.12392
  • Crystal, D. (1997). English as a global language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • De Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107. doi:10.1177/0956797614557866
  • Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., & Kinzler, K. D. (2012). The contribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: A neurodevelopmental study. Cerebral Cortex, 22(1), 209–220. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr111
  • Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1090–1097. doi:10.1177/0956797615574699
  • Gampe, A., Wermelinger, S., & Daum, M. M. (2019). Bilingual children adapt to the needs of their communication partners, monolinguals do not. Child Development, 90(1), 98–107. doi:10.1111/cdev.13190
  • Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1012–1020. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x
  • Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F. (2013). Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Nature Communications, 4(1), 1490. doi:10.1038/ncomms2430
  • Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(1), 1–15. doi:10.1017/S1366728903001007
  • Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36(1), 3–15. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(89)90048-5
  • Hamlin, J. K. (2013a). Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. Cognition, 128(3), 451–474. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.004
  • Hamlin, J. K. (2013b). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: Evidence for an innate moral core. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(3), 186–193. doi:10.1177/0963721412470687
  • Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: The role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 614. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00614
  • Hamlin, J. K., Ullman, T., Tenenbaum, J., Goodman, N., & Baker, C. (2013). The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: Experiments in preverbal infants and a computational model. Developmental Science, 16(2), 209–226. doi:10.1111/desc.12017
  • Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26(1), 30–39. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001
  • Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557–560. doi:10.1038/nature06288
  • Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13(6), 923–929. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00951.x
  • Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 19931–19936. doi:10.1073/pnas.1110306108
  • Brownell, C. A. (2010). Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Hollich, G. (2005). Supercoder: A program for coding preferential looking (Version 1.5). [Computer Software]. Indiana, USA, West Lafayette: Purdue University.
  • Ikizer, E. G., & Ramírez-Esparza, N. (2018). Bilinguals’ social flexibility. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 957–969. doi:10.1017/S1366728917000414
  • Incera, S., & McLennan, C. T. (2018). Bilingualism and age are continuous variables that influence executive function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 25(3), 443–463. doi:10.1080/13825585.2017.1319902
  • Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Language and the development of cognitive flexibility: Implications for theory of mind. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 144–162). New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0008
  • Killen, M., Mulvey, K. L., Richardson, C., Jampol, N., & Woodward, A. (2011). The accidental transgressor: Morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition, 119(2), 197–215. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.006
  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68(2), 263–277. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01939.x
  • Kovács, Á. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false‐belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 48–54. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.x
  • Kovács, Á. M., & Mehler, J. (2009a). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(16), 6556–6560. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811323106
  • Kovács, Á. M., & Mehler, J. (2009b). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325(5940), 611–612. doi:10.1126/science.1173947
  • Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychological Science, 14(5), 402–408. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.01454
  • Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12420. doi:10.1111/desc.12420
  • Baldwin, D. A., Moses, L. J., & Malle, B. F. (2001). Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of social cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2018). Infants’ evaluation of prosocial and antisocial agents: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 54(8), 1445–1455. doi:10.1037/dev0000538
  • Meristo, M., Strid, K., & Surian, L. (2016). Preverbal infants’ ability to encode the outcome of distributive actions. Infancy, 21(3), 353–372. doi:10.1111/infa.12124
  • Meristo, M., & Surian, L. (2013). Do infants detect indirect reciprocity? Cognition, 129(1), 102–113. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.006
  • Meristo, M., & Surian, L. (2014). Infants distinguish antisocial actions directed towards fair and unfair agents. PLoS One, 9(10), e110553. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0110553
  • Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002
  • Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 41–58. doi:10.1017/S0142716400009863
  • Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 567–579. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009
  • Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1997). Infants attribute value to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 848–856. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.84
  • Rubio-Fernández, P., & Glucksberg, S. (2012). Reasoning about other people’s beliefs: Bilinguals have an advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(1), 211–217. doi:10.1037/a0025162
  • Salvadori, E., Blazsekova, T., Volein, A., Karap, Z., Tatone, D., Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2015). Probing the strength of infants’ preference for helpers over hinderers: Two replication attempts of Hamlin and Wynn (2011). PLoS One, 10(11), e0140570. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0140570
  • Schmidt, M. F., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLoS One, 6(10), e23223. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023223
  • Schroeder, S. R. (2018). Do bilinguals have an advantage in theory of mind? A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Communication, 3. doi:10.3389/fcomm.2018.00036
  • Scola, C., Holvoet, C., Arciszewski, T., & Picard, D. (2015). Further evidence for infants’ preference for prosocial over antisocial behaviors. Infancy, 20(6), 684–692. doi:10.1111/infa.12095
  • Singh, L., Fu, C. S., Rahman, A. A., Hameed, W. B., Sanmugam, S., Agarwal, P., & Research Team, G. U. S. T. O. (2015). Back to basics: A bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development, 86(1), 294–302. doi:10.1111/cdev.12271
  • Singh, L., Loh, D., & Xiao, N. G. (2017). Bilingual infants demonstrate perceptual flexibility in phoneme discrimination but perceptual constraint in face discrimination. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1563. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01563
  • Singh, L., Quinn, P. C., Qian, M., & Lee, K. (2020). Bilingualism is associated with less racial bias in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 56(5), 888–896. doi:10.1037/dev0000905
  • Singh, L., Quinn, P. C., Xiao, N. G., & Lee, K. (2019). Monolingual but not bilingual infants demonstrate racial bias in social cue use. Developmental Science, e12809. doi:10.1111/desc.12809
  • Singh, L., Tan. A., Lee, K., & Quinn, P. C. (2020). Sensitivity to race in language comprehension in monolingual and bilingual infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 199, 104933. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104933
  • Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological Science, 23(2), 196–204. doi:10.1177/0956797611422072
  • Smetana, J. G., Jambon, M., Conry-Murray, C., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2012). Reciprocal associations between young children’s developing moral judgments and theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1144–1155. doi:10.1037/a0025891
  • Sommerville, J. A., Schmidt, M. F., Yun, J. E., & Burns, M. (2013). The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy, 18(1), 40–66. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00129.x
  • Van de Vondervoort, J. W., & Hamlin, J. K. (2016). Evidence for intuitive morality: Preverbal infants make sociomoral evaluations. Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 143–148. doi:10.1111/cdep.12175
  • Vera-Estay, E., Seni, A. G., Champagne, C., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2016). All for one: Contributions of age, socioeconomic factors, executive functioning, and social cognition to moral reasoning in childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 227. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00227
  • Verhagen, J., Grassmann, S., & Küntay, A. C. (2017). Monolingual and bilingual children’s resolution of referential conflicts: Effects of bilingualism and relative language proficiency. Cognitive Development, 41, 10–18. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.10.003
  • Völlm, B. A., Taylor, A. N., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., … Elliott, R. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage, 29(1), 90–98. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.022
  • Woo, B. M., Steckler, C. M., Le, D. T., & Hamlin, J. K. (2017). Social evaluation of intentional, truly accidental, and negligently accidental helpers and harmers by 10-month-old infants. Cognition, 168, 154–163. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.029
  • Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(20), 8235–8240. doi:10.1073/pnas.0701408104
  • Yow, W. Q., & Markman, E. M. (2011a). Young bilingual children’s heightened sensitivity to referential cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(1), 12–31. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.539524
  • Yow, W. Q., & Markman, E. M. (2011b). Bilingualism and children’s use of paralinguistic cues to interpret emotion in speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(4), 562–569. doi:10.1017/s1366728910000404
  • Ziv, T., & Sommerville, J. A. (2018). Developmental differences in infants’ fairness expectations from 6 to 15 months of age. Child Development, 88(6), 1930–1951. doi:10.1111/cdev.12674

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.