643
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Articles

Subtle Increments in Socioeconomic Status and Bilingualism Jointly Affect Children’s Verbal and Nonverbal Performance

&

References

  • Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 207–245. doi:10.3102/0034654310368803
  • Anderson, J. A. E., Hawrylewicz, K., & Bialystok, E. (2020). Who is bilingual? Snapshots across the lifespan. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 23(5), 929-937. doi:10.1017/S1366728918000950
  • Anton, E., Dunabeitia, J. A., Estevez, A., Hernandez, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., … Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 398. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398
  • Antoniou, M. (2019). The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annual Review of Linguistics, 5(1), 1–21. doi:10.1146/annurev-linguistics011718-011820
  • Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, S. (2005). The influence of the parents‘ educational level on the development of executive functions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(1), 539–560. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2801_5
  • Bak, T. H. (2016). The impact of bilingualism on cognitive ageing and dementia: Finding a path through a forest of confounding variables. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(1–2), 205–226. doi:10.1075/lab.15002.bak
  • Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 699–714. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003
  • Barac, R., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Behavioral and electrophysiological differences in executive control between monolingual and bilingual children. Child Development, 87(4), 1277–1290. doi:10.1111/cdev.12538
  • Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D. (2008). Assessment of bilingual children for identification of Language Impairment: Current findings and implications for practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(1), 1–29. doi:10.2167/beb392.0
  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007
  • Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 117–121. doi:10.1111/cdep.12116
  • Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. doi:10.1037/bul0000099
  • Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2007). Bilingualism and naming: Implications for cognitive assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(2), 209–211. doi:10.1017/S1355617707070403
  • Bialystok, E., & Luk, G. (2012). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingualism-Language and Cognition, 15(2), 397–401. doi:10.1017/S136672891100040X
  • Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531. doi:10.1017/S1366728909990423
  • Bialystok, E., & Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(1), 69–85. doi:10.1017/s0142716400010584
  • Blom, E., Kuntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128C, 128, 105–119. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007
  • Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256–269. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00974.x
  • Bornstein, M. H., Hendricks, C., Hahn, C. S., Haynes, O. M., Painter, K. M., & Tamis-lemonda, C. S. (2003). Contributors to self-perceived competence, satisfaction, investment, and role balance in maternal parenting: A multivariate ecological analysis. Parenting, Science and Practice, 3(4), 285–326. doi:10.1207/s15327922par0304_2
  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371–399. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233
  • Brito, N. H., & Noble, K. G. (2018). The independent and interacting effects of socioeconomic status and dual-language use on brain structure and cognition. Developmental Science, e12688. doi:10.1111/desc.12688
  • Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278–288. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015
  • Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x
  • Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Himel, C., & Bialystok, E. (2019). The impact of bilingualism on executive function in adolescents. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(6), 1278–1290. doi:10.1177/1367006918781059
  • Comishen, K., Bialystok, E., & Adler, S. A. (2019). The impact of bilingual environments on selective attention in infancy. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12797. doi:10.1111/desc.12797
  • Costa, A., Hernandez, A., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135–149. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
  • D’Souza, D., Brady, D., Haensel, J. X., & D’Souza, H. (2020). Is mere exposure enough? The effects of bilingual environments on infant cognitive development. Royal Society Open Science, 7(2), 180191. doi:10.1098/rsos.180191
  • Davis-Kean, P. E. (2005). The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 294–304. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.294
  • De Bruin, A. (2019). Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences, 9(3), 33. doi:10.3390/bs9030033
  • De Cat, C., Gusnanto, A., & Serratrice, L. (2017). Identifying a threshold for the executive function advantage in bilingual children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 119–151. doi:10.1017/S0272263116000486
  • Demir, Ö. E., & Küntay, A. C. (2014). Cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying socioeconomic gradients in language development: New answers to old questions. Child Development Perspectives, 8(2), 113–118. doi:10.1111/cdep.12069
  • Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J., & Homer, B. D. (2019). Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(4), 1122–1147. doi:10.3758/s13423-019-01567-z
  • Draheim, C., Mashburn, C. A., Martin, J. D., & Engle, R. W. (2019). Reaction time in differential and developmental research: A review and commentary on the problems and alternatives. Psychological Bulletin, 145(5), 508–535. doi:10.1037/bul0000192
  • Dunabeitia, J. A., Hernandez, J. A., Anton, E., Macizo, P., Estevez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234–251. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000243
  • Duncan, G. J., Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Kalil, A. (2010). Early childhood poverty and adult attainment, behavior, and health. Child Development, 81(1), 306–325. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01396.x
  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth Edition. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson Inc.
  • Engel De Abreu, P. M. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory, 19(5), 529–537. doi:10.1080/09658211.2011.590504
  • Engel De Abreu, P. M., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1364–1371. doi:10.1177/0956797612443836
  • Entwislea, D. R., & Astone, N. M. (1994). Some practical guidelines for measuring youth’s race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 65(6), 1521–1540. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00833.x
  • Esposito, A. G., Baker-Ward, L., & Mueller, S. (2013). Interference suppression vs. response inhibition: An explanation for the absence of a bilingual advantage in preschoolers’ Stroop task performance. Cognitive Development, 28(4), 354–363. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.09.002
  • Farah, M. J. (2017). The neuroscience of socioeconomic status: Correlates, causes, and consequences. Neuron, 96(1), 56–71. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.034
  • Farah, M. J., Shera, D. M., Savage, J. H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., … Hurt, H. (2006). Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research Reviews, 1110(1), 166–174. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.072
  • Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. doi:10.1111/desc.12019
  • Friesen, D. C., Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Proficiency and control in verbal fluency performance across the lifespan for monolinguals and bilinguals. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(3), 238–250. doi:10.1080/23273798.2014.918630
  • Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Vinas Guasch, N., … Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11.
  • Gatt, D., Baldacchino, R., & Dodd, B. (2020). Which measure of socioeconomic status best predicts bilingual lexical abilities and how? A focus on four-year-olds exposed to two majority languages. Journal of Child Language, 47(4), 737–765. doi:10.1017/S0305000919000886
  • Goriot, C., Broersma, M., McQueen, J. M., Unsworth, S., & Van Hout, R. (2018). Language balance and switching ability in children acquiring English as a second language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 168–186. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.019
  • Grogan, A., Green, D. W., Ali, N., Crinion, J. T., & Price, C. J. (2009). Structural correlates of semantic and phonemic fluency ability in first and second languages. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 2690–2698. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp023
  • Grundy, J. G. (2020). The effects of bilingualism on executive functions: An updated quantitative analysis. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 4, 177–199.
  • Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11(9), 651–659. doi:10.1038/nrn2897
  • Hair, N. L., Hanson, J. L., Wolfe, B. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2015). Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatric, 169(9), 822–829. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1475
  • Hartanto, A., Toh, W. X., & Yang, H. (2018). Bilingualism narrows socioeconomic disparities in executive functions and self-regulatory behaviors during early childhood: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study. Child Development, 90(4), 1215–1235. doi:10.1111/cdev.13032
  • Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7
  • Hoff‐Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother‐child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62(4), 782–796. doi:10.2307/1131177
  • Kang, C., Thoemmes, F., & Lust, B. (2016). Effects of SES on executive attention in Malay–English bilingual children in Singapore. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(5), 1042–1056. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000437
  • Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28(3), 233–246. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.011
  • Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 62(4), 6556–6560. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811323106
  • Krizman, J., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2016). Bilingual enhancements have no socioeconomic boundaries. Developmental Science, 19(6), 881–891. doi:10.1111/desc.12347
  • Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.795574
  • Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34. doi:10.1037/a0030875
  • Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114(1), 29–41. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014
  • Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81–93. doi:10.1017/S1366728907003227
  • Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavioral Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7
  • McCall, R. B. (1981). Early predictors of later IQ: The search continues. Intelligence, 5(2), 141–147. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(81)90004-0
  • Meir, N., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2017). Independent and combined effects of socioeconomic status (SES) and bilingualism on children’s vocabulary and verbal short-term memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1442. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01442
  • Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373–1386. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.x
  • Mistry, R. S., Biesanz, J. C., Chien, N., Howes, C., & Benner, A. D. (2008). Socioeconomic status, parental investments, and the cognitive and behavioral outcomes of low-income children from immigrant and native households. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(2), 193–212. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.01.002
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202.
  • Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x
  • Noble, K. G., Engelhardt, L. E., Brito, N. H., Mack, L. J., Nail, E. J., Angal, J., … Network, P. (2015). Socioeconomic disparities in neurocognitive development in the first two years of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(5), 535–551. doi:10.1002/dev.21303
  • Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., & Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science, 10(4), 464–480. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x
  • Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8(1), 74–87. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x
  • Noble, K. G., Wolmetz, M. E., Ochs, L. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Brain–behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental Science, 9(6), 642–654. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00542.x
  • Pace, A., Alper, R., Burchinal, M. R., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2019). Measuring success: Within and cross-domain predictors of academic and social trajectories in elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 112–125. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.001
  • Pena, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Kester, E. S. (2015). Assessment of language impairment in bilingual children using semantic tasks: Two languages classify better than one. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(2), 192–202. doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12199
  • Pino Escobar, G., Kalashnikova, M., & Escudero, P. (2018). Vocabulary matters! The relationship between verbal fluency and measures of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 170, 177–189. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.012
  • Poarch, G. J. (2018). Multilingual language control and executive function: A replication study. Frontiers in Communication, 3. doi:10.3389/fcomm.2018.0004
  • Poarch, G. J., & Van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535–551. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013
  • Pollak, S. D., & Wolfe, B. L. (2020). Maximizing research on the adverse effects of child poverty through consensus measures. Developmental Science, 23(6), e12946. doi:10.1111/desc.12946
  • Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 567–579. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009
  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2003). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
  • Revelle, W. (2020). psych: Procedures for personality and psychological research. Evanston, IL, USA: Northwestern University.
  • Sandoval, T. C., Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S., & Salmon, D. P. (2010). What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency? The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 231–252. doi:10.1017/S1366728909990514
  • Santillan, J., & Khurana, A. (2018). Developmental associations between bilingual experience and inhibitory control trajectories in Head Start children. Developmental Science, 21(4), e12624. doi:10.1111/desc.12624
  • Simon, J. R., & Wolf, J. D. (1963). Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6(1), 99–105. doi:10.1080/00140136308930679
  • Soliman, A. M. (2014). Bilingual advantages of working memory revisited: A latent variable examination. Learning and Individual Differences, 32, 168–177.
  • Sorge, G. B., Toplak, M. E., & Bialystok, E. (2017). Interactions between levels of attention ability and levels of bilingualism in children’s executive functioning. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12408. doi:10.1111/desc.12408
  • Statistics Canada. (2016a). GeoSearch tool used to identify neighborhood income from the census profile. Retrieved from https://tinyurl.com/ya5nqjom
  • Statistics Canada. (2016b). National occupational classification (Version 1.2). Retrieved from http://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb/p3VD.pl?Function=getVD&TVD=1204813
  • Statistics Canada. (2016c). Employment income statistics. Retrieved from https://tinyurl.com/y3pqkqfy
  • Statistics Canada. (2016d). 2016 Census of mother tongue for the residents of Canadian metropolitan areas. Retrieved from https://tinyurl.com/yxuny3rw
  • Team, R. (2020). RStudio: Integrated development for R. Boston, MA, USA: RStudio, PBC.
  • Thomas-Sunesson, D., Hakuta, K., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Degree of bilingualism modifies executive control in Hispanic children in the USA. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21(2), 197–206. doi:10.1080/13670050.2016.1148114
  • Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 412–422. doi:10.1017/S1366728910000611
  • Zeng, Z., Kalashnikova, M., & Antoniou, M. (2019). Integrating bilingualism, verbal fluency, and executive functioning across the lifespan. Journal of Cognition and Development, 20(5), 656–679. doi:10.1080/15248372.2019.1648267

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.