1,364
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Articles

Using Accuracy and Response Times to Assess Inhibitory Control in Kindergarten Children: An Analysis with Explanatory Item Response Models

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. New York, NY: Sage.
  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Berger, A., Jones, L., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Computerized games to study the development of attention in childhood. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32(2), 297–303. doi:10.3758/BF03207798
  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007
  • Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. doi:10.1037/bul0000099
  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10(1), 48–71. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(90)90004-N
  • Camerota, M., Willoughby, M. T., & Blair, C. B. (2019). Speed and accuracy on the hearts and flowers task interact to predict child outcomes. Psychological Assessment, 31(8), 995–1005. doi:10.1037/pas0000725
  • Camerota, M., Willoughby, M. T., Magnus, B. E., & Blair, C. B. (2020). Leveraging item accuracy and reaction time to improve measurement of child executive function ability. Psychological Assessment, 32(12), 1118–1132. doi:10.1037/pas0000953
  • Campbell, K. L., Lustig, C., Hasher, L., Campbell, K. L., Lustig, C., & Hasher, L. (2020). Aging and inhibition: The view ahead [Special issue]. Psychology and Aging, 35(5), 605–613. doi:10.1037/pag0000564
  • Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595–616. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3
  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333
  • Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(12), 547–552. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.005
  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
  • De Boeck, P., & Wilson, M., (Eds.). (2004). Explanatory item response models: A generalized linear and nonlinear approach. New York, NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-3990-9
  • Dempster, F. N. (1991). Inhibitory processes: A neglected dimension of intelligence. Intelligence, 15(2), 157–173. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(91)90028-C
  • Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85(5), 1777–1794. doi:10.1111/cdev.12237
  • Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive functions. In E. Bialystok & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 70–95). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195169539.003.0006
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
  • Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387–1388. doi:10.1126/science.1151148
  • Dixon, P. (2008). Models of accuracy in repeated measures designs. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 447–456. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.004
  • Dowsett, S. M., & Livesey, D. J. (2000). The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: Effects of “executive skills” training. Developmental Psychobiology, 36(2), 161–174. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(200003)36:2<161::AID-DEV7>3.0.CO;2-0
  • Duan, X., Wei, S., Wang, G., & Shi, J. (2010). The relationship between executive functions and intelligence on 11- to 12-year-old children. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52(4), 419–431.
  • Escobar, G. P., Kalashnikova, M., & Escudero, P. (2018). Vocabulary matters! The relationship between verbal fluency and measures of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 170, 177–189. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.012
  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6(6), 409–434. doi:10.1080/02699939208409696
  • Fischer, G. H. (1974). Einführung in die Theorie psychologischer Tests [Introduction to the theory of psychological tests]. Bern, Switzerland: Huber.
  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101
  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17(2), 172–179. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x
  • Garon, N. (2016). A review of hot executive functions in preschoolers. Journal of Self-Regulation and Regulation, 2, 57–80. doi:10.11588/josar.2016.2.34354
  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31–60. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
  • Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/2-7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X
  • Hartig, J., Frey, A., Nold, G., & Klieme, E. (2012). An application of explanatory item response modeling for model-based proficiency scaling. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 72(4), 665–686. doi:10.1177/0013164411430707
  • Heitz, R. P. (2014). The speed-accuracy tradeoff: History, physiology, methodology, and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 150. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00150
  • Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica, 26(2), 107–129. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(67)90011-X
  • Ikeda, Y., Okuzumi, H., & Kokubun, M. (2014). Age-related trends of inhibitory control in Stroop-like big-small task in 3 to 12-year-old children and young adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 227. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00227
  • Jones, L. B., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of executive attention in preschool children. Developmental Science, 6(5), 498–504. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00307
  • Kałamała, P., Szewczyk, J., Chuderski, A., Senderecka, M., & Wodniecka, Z. (2020). Patterns of bilingual language use and response inhibition: A test of the adaptive control hypothesis. Cognition, 204, Article 104373. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104373
  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 637–671. doi:10.3758/BF03196323
  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68(2), 263–277. doi:10.2307/1131849
  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67(2), 490–507. doi:10.2307/1131828
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. doi:10.18637/jss.v082.i13
  • Liesefeld, H. R., & Janczyk, M. (2019). Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs(?). Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 40–60. doi:10.3758/s13428-018-1076-x
  • Lipina, S., Segretin, S., Hermida, J., Prats, L., Fracchia, C., Camelo, J. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Linking childhood poverty and cognition: Environmental mediators of non-verbal executive control in an Argentine sample. Developmental Science, 16(5), 697–707. doi:10.1111/desc.12080
  • Long, J. A. (2019). Interactions: Comprehensive, user-friendly toolkit for probing interactions (R package version 1.1.0) [Computer Software].
  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
  • MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: Evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(1), 126–135. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.14.1.126
  • Magnus, B. E., Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., & Kuhn, L. J. (2019). Integrating item accuracy and reaction time to improve the measurement of inhibitory control abilities in early childhood. Assessment, 26(7), 1296–1306. doi:10.1177/1073191117740953
  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. doi:10.1177/0963721411429458
  • Montgomery, D. E., & Koeltzow, T. E. (2010). A review of the day–night task: The Stroop paradigm and interference control in young children. Developmental Review, 30(3), 308–330. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.07.001
  • Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2013). Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young children: A review of NIRS studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 867. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00867
  • Müller, U., & Kerns, K. (2015). The development of executive function. In L. S. Liben, U. Müller, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes (pp. 571–623). Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. doi:10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy214
  • Munakata, Y., Herd, S. A., Chatham, C. H., Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 453–459. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.011
  • Ng, F. F.-Y., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Yoshikawa, H., & Sze, I. N.-L. (2014). Inhibitory control in preschool predicts early math skills in first grade: Evidence from an ethnically diverse sample. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 39(2), 139–149. doi:10.1177/0165025414538558
  • Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220–246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220
  • Obradović, J. (2010). Effortful control and adaptive functioning of homeless children: Variable-focused and person-focused analyses [Editorial]. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 109–117. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2009.09.004
  • Paap, K. R. (2019). The bilingual advantage debate: Quantity and quality of the evidence. In J. W. Schwieter & M. Paradis (Eds.), The handbook of the neuroscience of multilingualism (pp. 701–735). Hoboken, New Jersey: Wiley-Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9781119387725.ch34
  • Petersen, I. T., Hoyniak, C. P., McQuillan, M. E., Bates, J. E., & Staples, A. D. (2016). Measuring the development of inhibitory control: The challenge of heterotypic continuity. Developmental Review, 40, 25–71. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2016.02.001
  • R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 4.0.3) [Computer Software]. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/
  • Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2009). The contribution of inhibitory control to preschoolers’ social–emotional competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 310–320. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.012
  • Roberts, R. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). An interactive framework for examining prefrontal cognitive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12(1), 105–126. doi:10.1080/87565649609540642
  • Roca, M., Parr, A., Thompson, R., Woolgar, A., Torralva, T., Antoun, N., … Duncan, J. (2010). Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 133(1), 234–247. doi:10.1093/brain/awp269
  • Roebers, C. M. (2017). Executive function and metacognition: Towards a unifying framework of cognitive self-regulation. Developmental Review, 45, 31–51. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2017.04.001
  • Schindler, J., Richter, T., Isberner, M.-B., Naumann, J., & Neeb, Y. (2018). Construct validity of a process-oriented test assessing syntactic skills in German primary schoolchildren. Language Assessment Quarterly, 15(2), 183–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/15434303.2018.1446142
  • Simpson, A., & Carroll, D. J. (2019). Understanding early inhibitory development: Distinguishing two ways that children use inhibitory control. Child Development, 90(5), 1459–1473. doi:10.1111/cdev.13283
  • Strommen, E. A. (1973). Verbal self-regulation in a children’s game: Impulsive errors on “Simon says” Child Development, 44(4), 849–853. doi:10.2307/1127737
  • Tamm, L., Menon, V., & Reiss, A. L. (2002). Maturation of brain function associated with response inhibition. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1231–1238. doi:10.1097/00004583-200210000-00013
  • Tellegen, P. J., Laros, J. A., & Petermann, F. (2006). Deutsche Normierung und Validierung des SON-R 2 ½ - 7 [Norming and validation of the SON-R 2 1/2 – 7 in German]. Bern, Switzerland: Hogrefe.
  • Ursache, A., & Raver, C. C. (2014). Trait and state anxiety: Relations to executive functioning in an at-risk sample. Cognition and Emotion, 28(5), 845–855. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.855173
  • Van Breukelen, G. J. P. (2005). Psychometric modeling of response speed and accuracy with mixed and conditional regression. Psychometrika, 70(2), 359–376. doi:10.1007/s11336-003-1078-0
  • Vandierendonck, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to combine speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the binning procedure. Behavior Research Methods, 49(2), 653–673. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0721-5
  • Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 418–424. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005
  • Verhagen, J., Mulder, H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2017). Effects of home language environment on inhibitory control in bilingual three-year-old children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(1), 114–127. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000590
  • Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 205–213. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.205
  • Willoughby, M. T., & Blair, C. B. (2016). Longitudinal measurement of executive function in preschoolers. In J. A. Griffin, P. McCardle, & L. S. Freund (Eds.), Executive function in preschool-age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research (pp. 91–113). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/14797-005
  • Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R. J., & Blair, C. B. (2011). Contributions of modern measurement theory to measuring executive function in early childhood: An empirical demonstration. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 414–435. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.007
  • Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R. J., & Blair, C. B. (2012). Executive function in early childhood: Longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychological Assessment, 24(2), 418–431. doi:10.1037/a0025779
  • Wilson, M., & De Boeck, P. (2004). Descriptive and explanatory item response models. In P. De Boeck & M. Wilson (Eds.), Explanatory item response models: A generalized linear and nonlinear approach (pp. 43–74). New York, NY: Springer.
  • Yücel, M., Fornito, A., Youssef, G., Dwyer, D., Whittle, S., Wood, S. J., … Allen, N. B. (2012). Inhibitory control in young adolescents: The role of sex, intelligence, and temperament. Neuropsychology, 26(3), 347–356. doi:10.1037/a0027693
  • Zhao, X., Chen, L., & Maes, J. H. R. (2018). Training and transfer effects of response inhibition training in children and adults. Developmental Science, 21(1), 1–12. doi:10.1111/desc.12511