1,027
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Articles

The Origins of Theory of Mind in Infant Social Cognition: Investigating Longitudinal Pathways from Intention Understanding and Joint Attention to Preschool Theory of Mind

&

References

  • Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). Joint attention and vocabulary development: A critical look. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(3), 195–207. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00014.x
  • Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2008). On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives, 2(2), 59–65. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00044.x
  • Aschersleben, G., Hofer, T., & Jovanovic, B. (2008). The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. Developmental Science, 11(6), 862–868. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00736.x
  • Astington, J. W., & Baird, N. A. (Eds.). (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55(4), 1278–1289. doi:10.2307/1129997
  • Bakker, M., Van Dijk, A., & Wicherts, J. M. (2012). The rules of the game called psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 543–554. doi:10.1177/1745691612459060
  • Bard, K. A., Keller, H., Ross, K. M., Hewlett, B., Butler, L., Boysen, S. T., & Matsuzawa, T. (2021). Joint attention in human and chimpanzee infants in varied socio ecological contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 86(4), 7–217. doi:10.1111/mono.12435
  • Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(1), 107–122. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00041790
  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Besiroglu, B., Alayli, A., Tahiroglu, E., & Selcuk, B. (2022). The psychometric properties of the children’s social understanding scale among atypically developing children and adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 31(8), 2275–2291. doi:10.1007/s10826-022-02238-9
  • Białecka-Pikul, M., & Stępień-Nycz, M. (2019). Parent-report measure of theory of mind: Adaptation to polish of the children’s social understanding scale. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(3), 318–326. doi:10.1080/17405629.2017.1406850
  • Brandone, A. C. (2015). Infants’ social and motor experience and the emerging understanding of intentional actions. Developmental Psychology, 51(4), 512. doi:10.1037/a0038844
  • Brandone, A. C., Horwitz, S. R., Aslin, R. N., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). Infants’ goal anticipation during failed and successful reaching actions. Developmental Science, 17(1), 23–34. doi:10.1111/desc.12095
  • Brandone, A. C., Stout, W., & Moty, K. (2020). Triadic interactions support infants’ emerging understanding of intentional actions. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12880. doi:10.1111/desc.12880
  • Brandone, A. C., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). You can’t always get what you want: Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychological Science, 20(1), 85–91. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02246.x
  • Bretherton, I. (1991). Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. In D. Frye & C. Moore (Eds.), Children’s theories of mind (pp. 49–75). New York: Erlbaum.
  • Brink, K. A., Lane, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (2015). Developmental pathways for social understanding: Linking social cognition to social contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 719. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00719
  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: A longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 67–78. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.010
  • Brune, C. W., & Woodward, A. L. (2007). Social cognition and social responsiveness in 10-month-old infants. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(2), 133–158. doi:10.1080/15248370701202331
  • Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365–376. doi:10.1038/nrn3475
  • Cannon, E. N., & Woodward, A. L. (2012). Infants generate goal-based action predictions. Developmental Science, 15(2), 292–298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01127.x
  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333
  • Carpendale, J. I., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 79–96. doi:10.1017/S0140525X04000032
  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4, Serial No. 255). doi:10.2307/1166214
  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 481–498. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00037-5
  • Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231–239. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007
  • Colonnesi, C., Rieffe, C., Koops, W., & Perucchini, P. (2008). Precursors of a theory of mind: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 561–577. doi:10.1348/026151008X285660
  • Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta analysis. Child Development, 85(5), 1777–1794. doi:10.1111/cdev.12237
  • Dunphy-Lelii, S., LaBounty, J., Lane, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). The social context of infant intention understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 60–77. doi:10.1080/15248372.2012.710863
  • Ekerim Akbulut, M., Etel, E., Tahirolu, D., & Selcuk, A. B. (2021). Children’s social understanding scale-short form: Adaptation to Turkish sample. Early Education and Development doi:10.1080/10409289.2021.2013668
  • Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., & Von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9(7), 878–879. doi:10.1038/nn1729
  • Filippi, C., Choi, Y. B., Fox, N. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2020). Neural correlates of infant action processing relate to theory of mind in early childhood. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12876. doi:10.1111/desc.12876
  • Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165–193. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-H
  • Gluck, S. C., Tahiroglu, D., & Moses, L. J. (2020). A comparison of mother and father reports of children’s theory of mind: Further validation of the children’s social understanding scale. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1–11. doi:10.1080/17405629.2020.1813100
  • Gredebäck, G., Johnson, S., & von Hofsten, C. (2009). Eye tracking in infancy research. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(1), 1–19. doi:10.1080/87565640903325758
  • Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 1026. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.1026
  • Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2010). The early development of object knowledge: A study of infants’ visual anticipations during action observation. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 446. doi:10.1037/a0016543
  • Kloo, D., Kristen-Antonow, S., & Sodian, B. (2020). Progressing from an implicit to an explicit false belief understanding: A matter of executive control? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 44(2), 107–115. doi:10.1177/0165025419850901
  • Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Perst, H. (2011). Infants’ joint attention skills predict toddlers’ emerging mental state language. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1207. doi:10.1037/a0024808
  • Krogh Jespersen, S., Liberman, Z., & Woodward, A. L. (2015). Think fast! The relationship between goal prediction speed and social competence in infants. Developmental Science, 18, 815–823. doi:10.1111/desc.12249
  • Laible, D., Karahuta, E., Stout, W., Van Norden, C., Cruz, A., Neely, P., … Agalar, A. E. (2021). Toddlers’ helping, sharing, and empathic distress: Does the race of the target matter? Developmental Psychology, 57(9), 1452–1462. doi:10.1037/dev0001233
  • Liszkowski, U. (2013). Using theory of mind. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 104–109. doi:10.1111/cdep.12025
  • Malesa, E., Foss-Feig, J., Yoder, P., Warren, Z., Walden, T., & Stone, W. L. (2013). Predicting language and social outcomes at age 5 for later-born siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 17(5), 558–570. doi:10.1177/1362361312444628
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838–850. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
  • Miller, S. E., & Marcovitch, S. (2015). Examining executive function in the second year of life: Coherence, stability, and relations to joint attention and language. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 101. doi:10.1037/a0038359
  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta‐analysis of the relation between language ability and false‐belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x
  • Mundy, P. (2016). Autism and joint attention: Development, neuroscience, and clinical fundamentals. New York: Guilford Publications.
  • Mundy, P., & Acra, C. F. (2006). Joint attention, social engagement, and the development of social competence. In P. J. Marshall & N. Fox (Eds.), Development of social engagement: Neurobiological perspectives (pp. 81–117). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x
  • Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. (2000). EEG correlates of the development of infant joint attention skills. Developmental Psychobiology, 36(4), 325–338. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(200005)36:4<325::AID-DEV7>3.0.CO;2-f
  • Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged early social communication scales. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami.
  • Mundy, P., & Jarrold, W. (2010). Infant joint attention, neural networks and social cognition. Neural Networks, 23(8), 985–997. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.009
  • Mundy, P., & Newell, L. (2007). Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 269–274. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00518.x
  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2020). Mplus Version 8.5.
  • Nelson, P. B., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2008). Toddlers’ joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers’ acquisition of theory of mind. Developmental Science, 11(6), 847–852. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00733.x
  • Oakes, L. M. (2017). Sample size, statistical power, and false conclusions in infant looking-time research. Infancy, 22(4), 436–469. doi:10.1111/infa.12186
  • Olineck, K. M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2007). Imitation of intentional actions and internal state language in infancy predict preschool theory of mind skills. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(1), 14–30. doi:10.1080/17405620601046931
  • Poulin-Dubois, D., Azar, N., Elkaim, B., & Burnside, K. (2020). Testing the stability of theory of mind: A longitudinal approach. Plos one, 15(11), e0241721. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0241721
  • R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
  • Redcay, E., Kleiner, M., & Saxe, R. (2012). Look at this: The neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 169. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00169
  • Resendes, T., Benchimol-Elkaim, B., Delisle, C., Rene, J.-L., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2021). What I know and what you know: The role of metacognitive strategies in preschoolers’ selective social learning. Cognitive Development, 60, 101117. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101117
  • Sheinkopf, S., Mundy, P., Claussen, A., & Willoughby, J. (2004). Infant joint attention skill and preschool behavioral outcomes in at-risk children. Development and Psychopathology, 16(2), 273–291. doi:10.1017/S0954579404044517
  • Slaugher, V., & McConnell, D. (2003). Emergence of joint attention: Relationships between gaze following, social referencing, imitation, and naming in infancy. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164(1), 54–71. doi:10.1080/00221320309597503
  • Smogorzewska, J., Szumsi, G., & Grygiel, P. (2019). The children’s social understanding scale: An advanced analysis of a parent-report measure for assessing theory of mind in Polish children with and without disabilities. Developmental Psychology, 55(4), 835–845. doi:10.1037/dev0000673
  • Sodian, B., Kristen‐Antonow, S., & Kloo, D. (2020). How does children’s theory of mind become explicit? A review of longitudinal findings. Child Development Perspectives, 14(3), 171–177. doi:10.1111/cdep.12381
  • Sodian, B., & Kristen-Antonow, S. (2015). Declarative joint attention as a foundation of theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 51(9), 1190. doi:10.1037/dev0000039
  • Sodian, B., Licata, M., Kristen‐Antonow, S., Paulus, M., Killen, M., & Woodward, A. (2016). Understanding of goals, beliefs, and desires predicts morally relevant theory of mind: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 87(4), 1221–1232. doi:10.1111/cdev.12533
  • Stout, W., Karahuta, E., Laible, D., & Brandone, A. C. (2021). A longitudinal study of the differential social‐cognitive foundations of early prosocial behaviors. Infancy, 26(2), 271–290. doi:10.1111/infa.12381
  • Tahiroglu, D., Moses, L. J., Carlson, S. M., Mahy, C. E., Olofson, E. L., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2014). The children’s social understanding scale: Construction and validation of a parent-report measure for assessing individual differences in children’s theories of mind. Developmental Psychology, 50(11), 2485. doi:10.1037/a0037914
  • Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H., Kristen, S., Tahiroglu, D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 172–187. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x
  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). New York: Psychology Press.
  • Tomasello, M. (2018). How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(34), 8491–8498. doi:10.1073/pnas.1804761115
  • Tompkins, V. (2022). Relations between the home literacy environment and young children’s theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 62, 101179. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101179
  • Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P. C., Acra, C. F., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E., Parlade, M. V., … Pomares, Y. B. (2007). Infant joint attention, temperament, and social competence in preschool children. Child Development, 78(1), 53–69. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00985.x
  • Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E., Parlade, M. V., Pomares, Y. B., & Hobson, J. A. (2012). Infant responding to joint attention, executive processes, and self-regulation in preschool children. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(2), 303–311. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.12.001
  • Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta‐analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304
  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory‐of‐mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
  • Wellman, H. M., Lopez-Duran, S., LaBounty, J., & Hamilton, B. (2008). Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. Developmental Psychology 44(2), 618–623. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.618
  • Wellman, H. M., Phillips, A. T., Dunphy‐Lelii, S., & LaLonde, N. (2004). Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. Developmental Science, 7(3), 283–288. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00347.x
  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
  • Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
  • Woodward, A. L., Sommerville, J. A., Gerson, S., Henderson, A. M., & Buresh, J. (2009). The emergence of intention attribution in infancy. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 187–222. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(09)51006-7
  • Yamaguchi, M., Kuhlmeier, V. A., Wynn, K., & VanMarle, K. (2009). Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood. Developmental Science, 12(5), 746–752. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.